• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Interchange between grooming and infant handling in female Tibetan macaques(Macaca thibetana)

    2019-04-29 07:10:34QiJiangDongPoXiaXiWangDaoZhangBingHuaSunJinHuaLi
    Zoological Research 2019年2期

    Qi Jiang,Dong-Po Xia,Xi Wang,Dao Zhang,Bing-Hua Sun,Jin-Hua Li,3,*

    1School of Resource and Environmental Engineering,Anhui University,Hefei Anhui 230601,China

    2School of Life Science,Anhui University,Hefei Anhui 230601,China

    3School of Life Science,Hefei Normal University,Hefei Anhui 241000,China

    ABSTRACT In some non-human primates,infants function as a social tool that can bridge relationships among group members.Infants are a desired commodity for group members,and mothers control access to them.The biological market theory suggests that grooming is widespread and represents a commodity that can be exchanged for infant handling. As a limited resource,however,the extent to which infants are interchanged between mothers(females with an infant)and non-mothers(potential handlers,females without an infant)remains unclear.In this study,we collected behavioral data to investigate the relationship between grooming and infant handling in free-ranging Tibetan macaques(Macaca thibetana)at Mt.Huangshan,China.Our results showed that females with infants received more grooming than females without infants.After her infant was handled,mother females received more grooming than they did during daily grooming interactions.However,with the increasing number of infants within the social group,both the grooming that mothers received and the grooming that non-mothers invested for handling infants decreased.We also found that non-mothers invested more time in grooming to gain access to younger infants than older infants.Our results provide evidence that infants are social commodities for both mother and non-mother females.Mothers use infants for obtain grooming and non-mothers use grooming to gain access to infants.The current study implies a bidirectional and complex interchange pattern between grooming and infant handling to compensate for the dyadic grooming disparity in non-human primates.

    Keywords:Tibetan macaques(Macaca thibetana);Interchange;Infant handling;Grooming;Biological market theory

    INTRODUCTION

    While female attraction to infants represents a common feature in non-human primate species,maternal responses to infant handling show a certain degree of variability(Maestripieri,1994;Nicolson,1987). In some species,such as Asian colobines,females allow their newborn infants to be held frequently and carried for long durations by other group members(infant caretaking)(Nicolson,1987;Stanford,1992).In other species,such as baboons and macaques,mothers are much more restrictive in allowing access their young offspring(Altmann,2002;Nicolson,1987),despite persistent attempts by group members to interact with their infant(Frank&Silk,2009;Gumert,2007a;Henzi&Barrett,2002).It is well documented that infants can be used as social tools to buffer agonistic interactions(Dunbar,1984;Taub,1980)and to facilitate social bonds(Dunayer&Berman,2017;Hsu et al.,2016;Manson,1999).Therefore,the infant handling concept may be indicative of the behavioral pattern that exists between an infant and a non-mother group member(Hsu et al.,2016;Jin et al.,2015;Maestripieri,1994).

    In recent decades,studies on the social function of infant handling within the framework of the biological market theory have gained increasing interest(Ueno,2017;Wei et al.,2013;Yu et al.,2013).Biological market theory proposes that the infant can be considered as a social commodity,and thus interchange relationships between infant handling and behavioral commodities may exist. For example,in chacma baboons(Papio cynocephalus ursinus),Henzi&Barrett(2002)proposed that female A will groom female B more often if female B has a newborn infant in order to gain access.Yu et al.(2013)described similar findings in golden snub-nosed monkeys(Rhinopithecus roxellana),indicating that infant handling might be interchanged for grooming.Infant handling is also reported as a social commodity to interchange for Affiliation,e.g.,embracing in spider monkeys(Ateles geoffroyi yucatanensis)(Slater et al.,2007).

    Grooming is a measure of Affiliative social relationships among non-human primates(Dunbar,2010).In some species,grooming accounts for as much as 10%–20%of an individual’s total daily activity budget(Lehmann et al.,2007).Previous studies have demonstrated that grooming can be interchanged for support in agonistic encounters(Seyfarth&Cheney,1984),mating opportunities(Barrett&Henzi,2001;Gumert,2007b),tolerance(Gumert&Ho,2008),food(Watts,2002),and infant handling(Gumert,2007a;Henzi&Barrett,2002;Tiddi et al.,2010).In tufted capuchin monkeys(Cebus apella nigritus),Tiddi et al.(2010)proposed that grooming is interchanged for infant handling among females,with grooming given by potential handlers increasing the likelihood of subsequent infant handling opportunities.Similar results have also been found in wild baboons(P.anubis)(Frank&Silk,2009),long-tail macaques(Macaca fascicularis)(Gumert,2007a),and golden snub-nosed monkeys(Wei et al.,2013).

    These previous studies suggest that both grooming and infant handling opportunities are valuable commodities in primates.Infant handling consists of an infant and adult female other than its mother.The groomer(female A)may interchange grooming to gain access to the groomee’s infant(female B,the infant’s mother). Accordingly,as a limited-competition resource,infants could be interchanged between the groomee(supplier,infant’s mother)and groomer(demander,potential handler,non-mother).However,the extent to which individuals perform grooming for infant handling or provide infant handling for grooming among females remains unclear.

    We used the biological market perspective to examine the interchange relationships between grooming and infant handling in Tibetan macaques(M.thibetana). Tibetan macaques live in multi-male-multi-female social groups(mean group size=33.8±6.79),with female philopatry and male dispersion(Li,1999).Tibetan macaques devote approximately 20%of their daily activity budget to grooming and thus it can be considered as a behavioral commodity in their social groups(Xia et al.,2012,2013).Similar to other macaque species,group members persistently attempt to interact with infants in multiple ways(Li,1999).

    We hypothesized the existence of bidirectional and complex interchange patterns between grooming and infant handling in Tibetan macaques.We tested the following predictions:if an infant is a limited and valuable commodity,(1)females with infants will be more attractive than females without infants,and females will receive more but provide less grooming after giving birth than before parturition;(2)with an increase in the number of infants,females with infants will receive less grooming from females without infants;(3)females with infants will receive more grooming if they first allow their infant to be handled;if grooming is an effective way to gain access to an infant,(4)females will obtain more infant handling opportunities after grooming the infant’s mother than they have under baseline social conditions.

    MATERIALS AND METHODS

    Study site and subjects

    This study was conducted at Mt.Huangshan National Reserve in Anhui Province,China.The reserve is a World Culture and Nature Heritage site and well-known tourist destination,as well as research site for the study of Tibetan macaques.

    The study site can be found within the reserve in an area known as the “Valley of the Monkeys”.There are two groups at the site:Yulingkeng A1(YA1,target group in this study)and Yulingkeng A2(YA2).Matrilineal kinship is known from historic and demographic data collected daily since 1986(Li,1999). The target group has been thoroughly habituated to close observation(i.e.,from<1 m)and all individuals can be recognized using physical features(i.e.,scars,hair color patterns,or facial/body contours)without disturbance or capture.During the study period,the group consisted of eleven adult males,thirteen adult females, five subadults/juveniles,thirteen yearlings,and eleven infants≤6-months-old(Table 1).We selected thirteen adult females and two subadult females for this study.

    Table 1 Birth records of infants in the YA1 group during the study period

    Data collection

    This study was conducted from July 2016 to January 2017 and from March to May 2017. All behavioral data were collected during an intensive study period over 201 days(average=25.1day/month,range=24–27). The social group was followed from dawn to dusk,and behavioral observations began at approximately 0700–0800 h and ended at 1700–1800 h each day(depending on the time of year).The observer maintained an observation distance of 5–10 m from the monkeys.

    Focal animal sampling and continuous recording(using a digital voice recorder)were used to score the daily activity of the focal individual(Altmann,1974),with the recorded data used as the baseline.Focal sample duration was set at 15 min(Li et al.,2007).We followed the sampling rules of Xia et al. (2012,2013)to avoid the influence of humans and double-counting social interactions.

    To investigate whether non-mothers were more likely to handle infants after grooming their mothers,we collected post-grooming(PG)samples. The PG samples consisted of 15-min focal observations of a non-mother who had just groomed a mother.To investigate whether infant handling by non-mothers increased subsequent grooming investment given to mothers,we collected post-infant handling(PH)samples.The PH samples consisted of 15-min focal observations of a non-mother who had just handled an infant of a mother.On the next day,at the same time,we conducted 15-min matched control(MC)focal observations matched with a PG or PH sample,with no grooming or infant handling preceding the focal observation(De Waal&Yoshihara,1983).We collected MC samples only if the two participants were in proximity(within 5 m).If the two participants were not in proximity during the MC observations and/or were involved in handling or grooming interactions within the 5 min preceding a planned MC or in the first 5 min of an ongoing MC,we postponed the MC until all conditions were meet.If a MC observation could not be conducted within one week of the PG or PH,the PG or PH was discarded.

    We used ad libitum sampling to collect data on duration of all grooming bouts between mothers and non-mothers that involved infant handling.We categorized an interaction as a grooming-infant handling interchange if a non-mother groomed a mother and handled her infant.This could occur in any order and any number of times.The interaction finished when the two individuals moved>5 m away from each other.We measured the total duration of grooming until the two females departed each other. We recorded the dyadic social relationships immediately when an interaction ended.According to Xia et al.(2012;2013),we defined grooming as any act in which a macaque(groomer)used its hand or mouth to touch,clean,or manipulate the fur of another individual(groomee).Infant handling behaviors included inspect,teeth-chatter,hold,groom,and bridge(Table 2).

    We used ad libitum sampling to record aggressive and submissive behaviors to determine dominance relationships.Aggressive interactions were defined as an individual threatening,chasing,slapping,grabbing,or biting another individual(Berman et al.,2007).Submissive behaviors were scored when an individual showed fearful interactions,such as fear grin,cower,mock leave,avoid, fl ee,or scream(Berman et al.,2004). All records of agonism were tallied for each focal female and divided into aggression given and aggression received.

    Data analysis

    We reported baseline data as mean±SE grooming duration(min/h)and infant handling frequency(times/h).We used a one sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test to examine whether the data conformed to normal distribution(P>0.05).

    Table 2 Definitions of infant handling behaviors

    We used the baseline data and calculated the duration of grooming received by mothers from non-mothers and given to non-mothers by mothers before parturition and six months after giving birth.Data were compared using paired t-tests to analyze whether mothers were groomed for longer periods after giving birth than before parturition,and whether mothers groomed non-mothers less after giving birth than before parturition.

    We collected data on 11 of the 15 females investigated for PG and PH.No data were collected for three adult females(TT,YZ,YM),who were older and less socially active in regard to infant handling and did not give birth or become pregnant during the study.One subadult female(THX)was not observed to engage in a grooming-infant handling interchange.We used paired t-test to compare the duration of grooming between focal samples and PH samples,and between MC samples and PH samples to determine if non-mothers handling infants promoted the non-mothers to groom mothers.We used paired t-test to compare the frequency of infant handling between focal samples and PG samples,and between MC samples and PG samples to determine if non-mothers grooming mothers promoted non-mother infant handling.

    We used the ad libitum data collected on grooming-infant handling interchange to investigate whether the number of infants per female was negatively correlated with duration of non-mothers grooming mothers during the interaction. We used linear regression to determine relationships between grooming and the number of infants per female.

    We analyzed grooming-infant handling interchange data to elucidate the effect of infant age on the duration of non-mothers grooming mothers during the interaction. We used linear regression to determine the relationship between infant age and duration of non-mothers grooming mothers.We assessed individual dominance rank by calculating David’s Score(DS).We also calculated linearity for the obtained dominance hierarchy(De Vries,1995;Gammell et al.,2003)Rank distance is the number of individuals ranking between the focal animal and a given partner,plus 1(Castles et al.,1999). In our study,the rank distance was the number of individuals ranking between the mother and non-mother,using the mother’s rank as the standard.We determined the sequence of social ranks based on the DS values,according to Gammell et al.(2003).YH was the highest ranked,followed by YXX,YCY,YM,TXH,TH,TXX,YCLA,HH,TR,TRY,THX,TT,YZ,and THY.

    We analyzed grooming-infant handling interchange data and used linear regression analysis to test the relationship between rank distance and duration of non-mothers grooming mothers.To investigate whether kinship between non-mother and mother affected the duration of non-mothers grooming mothers when infant handling,we used paired t-tests to compare the duration of non-mothers grooming mothers between kin and non-kin.

    To account for potential bias caused by pseudoreplication,we used factorial ANOVA to test for variation across individuals and number of infants,infant age,and rank distance on the duration of non-mothers grooming mothers. We found a significant effect of number of infants,infant age,and rank distance on grooming duration,but no significant effect on individuals or interaction effects. We found no evidence that individual variation existed and therefore no support that individual idiosyncrasies solely accounted for the results.Thus,pseudoreplication did not seriously affect the results.

    RESULTS

    During the study period,we collected 90 valid PG-MC samples,86 valid PH-MC samples,and 148 grooming-infant handling interchanges.

    Grooming variation among females

    The mean duration ofmothersgrooming non-mothers before birth(1.04±0.05 min/h)(mean±SE)was significantly longerthan afterbirth (0.62±0.07 min/h)(paired t-test:t=6.233,df=5,P=0.002),whereas the mean duration of non-mothers grooming mothers after birth(1.27±0.17 min/h)was significantly longer than that before birth(0.66±0.09 min/h)(paired t-test:t=–4.218,df=5,P=0.008)(Figure 1).

    Grooming variation between PH and baseline

    Forfocalsample observations,the mean duration of non-mothers grooming mothers was 1.52±0.13 min/h(mean±SE).After non-mothers handled infants of mothers,the mean duration of non-mothers grooming mothers was 3.02±0.22 min/h(mean±SE).The duration of grooming was significantly higher in PH samples than in focal samples(paired t-test:t=7.679,df=10,P<0.001).In MC sample observations,the mean duration of non-mothers grooming mothers was 1.74±0.08 min/h(mean±SE).The duration of grooming was significantly higher in PH samples than in MC samples(paired t-test:t=6.475,df=10,P<0.001)(Figure 2).

    Figure 1 Duration of grooming females gave and received before birth and after birth(mean±SE)

    Figure 2 Duration of non-mothers grooming mothers(mean±SE)

    Grooming variation with number of infants

    The number of infants per female was negatively correlated with the duration of non-mothers grooming mothers during grooming-infanthandling interchange (linear regression:R=0.624,F=93.271,P<0.001,R2=0.390,df=147)(Figure 3).

    Variation of infant handling

    In focalsample observations,the mean frequency of non-mother infant handling was 2.07±0.48 times/h(mean±SE).After non-mothers groomed mothers,the mean frequency of non-mother infant handling was 6.20±0.65 times/h(mean±SE).The mean frequency of infant handling was significantly higher in PG samples than in focal samples(paired t-test:t=7.642,df=10,P<0.001).In MC sample observations,the mean frequency of infant handling was 2.60±0.36 times/h(mean±SE).The mean frequency of infant handling was significantly higher in PG samples than in MC samples(paired t-test:t=5.858,df=10,P<0.001)(Figure 4).

    Figure 3 Duration of non-mothers grooming mothers was negatively correlated with the number of infants per female(mean±SE)

    Figure 4 Frequency of non-mother infant handling(mean±SE)

    Effect of infant age

    Infantage was negativelycorrelated with the duration of non-mothers grooming mothers during grooming-infant handling interchanges(linear regression:R=0.634,F=98.128,P<0.001,R2=0.402,df=147)(Figure 5). The duration of non-mothers grooming mothers decreased significantly with infant age.

    Effect of rank and kin

    The rank distance between mothers and non-mothers was negatively correlated with the duration ofnon-mothers grooming mothers(linear regression:R=0.277,F=12.088,P=0.001,R2=0.076,df=147)(Figure 6).

    There was no significant difference in the duration of non-mothers grooming mothers when infant handling between kin and non-kin(paired t-test:t=0.758,df=4,P=0.491).

    Figure 5 Duration of non-mothers grooming mothers was negatively correlated with infant age(mean±SE)

    Figure 6 Duration of non-mothers grooming mothers was negatively correlated with rank distance between mothers and non-mothers(mean±SE)

    DISCUSSION

    Infants are considered as commodities between a groomee(supplier,infant’s mother)and groomer(demander,potential handler,non-mother).However,the extent to which individuals groom in exchange for infant handling or offer infant handling in exchange for grooming among females remains unclear.In this study,we found that mothers received more grooming from non-mothers after birth than before birth,and that mothers groomed non-mothers less after birth than before birth.These results supported the prediction that females with infants are more attractive than females without infants and that females will receive more but provide less grooming after birth than before parturition.These findings are similar to that reported for golden snub-nosed monkeys(R.roxellana)(Wei et al.,2013).Furthermore,after non-mothers handled infants,the duration of non-mothers grooming mothers was significantly higher than that observed under baseline conditions,supporting the prediction that females with infants will receive more grooming if they first allow their infant to be handled. These results are similar to that reported for wild baboons(P.anubis)(Frank&Silk,2009). We also demonstrated positive evidence for the prediction that females will gain higher infant handling opportunities after grooming an infant’s mother compared with baseline interactions. Similar results have also been found in studies on golden snub-nosed monkeys(R.roxellana)and long-tailed macaques(M.fascicularis)(Gumert,2007a;Yu et al.,2013).Thus,we concluded that infants are a limited and valuable commodity and can be used by mothers in exchange for grooming.Furthermore,grooming is an effective way for non-mothers to access infants.Our results showed bidirectional and complex interchange patterns between grooming and infant handling in Tibetan macaques,and that the adaptive investment of females will change based on their social characteristics and group dynamics.

    A single behavioral exchange may not explain all behavioral relationships among individuals.In the case of reciprocity,pairs of actors are expected to form long-term and predictable social relationships so that,on average,the overall costs and benefits to each are relatively equal over time(Seyfarth,1977;Xia et al.,2012).In contrast,under conditions in which individual actors have the opportunity to interact with a diverse set of social partners who vary in the quality of goods or services each can provide at any given moment in time,individuals are expected to seek out partners who provide the greatest benefit at the lowest cost(No? et al.,1991;Wilkinson,1984).These interactions can involve an exchange of the same goods or services(i.e.,grooming in exchange for grooming)or the interchange of different goods or services.In Tibetan macaques,grooming can be exchanged for grooming(Xia et al.,2012,2013)and interchanged for tolerance(Xia et al.,2012). In our study,grooming was interchanged for infant handling.However,we suggest that a single exchange may not fully explain the behavioral relationship between individuals.The behavioral exchange model is complex,reflecting the evolution of individual adaptability and promoting relationship maintenance and group stability in primate societies.

    Market forces affect the interchange between grooming and infant handling. Infant age is a market force that can influence grooming payment. In our study,Tibetan macaque females groom other females for a longer period when theyhave a youngerinfant,with similarresults also reported in studies on long-tailed macaques (M.fascicularis),tufted capuchin monkeys(Cebus apella nigritus),sooty mangabeys(Cercocebus atys),and vervet monkeys(Chlorocebus aethiops)(Fruteau et al.,2011;Gumert,2007a;Tiddi et al.,2010).These results reflect that newborn infants are generally more attractive than older infants(Nicolson,1987),with infants get older,their attractiveness declined.Market theory predicts that supply and demand will influence behavior exchange among group members by altering the value of the commodities traded(No? et al.,2001).Under a biological market perspective,potential handlers attracted to infants represent the demand and infants represent the supply(Gumert,2007a;Henzi&Barrett,2002).In Tibetan macaques,with the increase in the number of infants,the duration of non-mothers grooming mothers for infant handling decreased significantly. Similar results have been reported in studies on chacma baboons(P.cynocephalus ursinus)and long-tailed macaques(M.fascicularis)(Gumert,2007a;Henzi&Barrett,2002).

    Dominance hierarchy is a factor that has considerable influence on socialinterchange in non-human primates.Dominance relationshipsmaymake socialcommodities cheaper for higher-ranked individuals and more expensive for lower-ranked individuals(Gumert,2007a;Henzi&Barrett,2002;Tiddi et al.,2010;Wei et al.,2012).In Tibetan macaques,rank can affect the patterns of grooming reciprocity among females;within the context of a biological market,higher ranking females have a wider set of social options than middle and lower ranking females(Xia et al.,2012). Our study indicated that dominance also altered the interchange between grooming and infant handling in Tibetan macaques,by reducing the grooming investment that higher ranking non-mothers gave to mothers when infant handling and increasing the grooming investment that lower ranking non-mothers gave to mothers.These results indicate that,in an infant market,lower-ranked individuals will pay more for interacting with higher-ranked partners,and rank might corrupt a social market and skew social exchange to benefit higher-ranked individuals.

    In Tibetan macaques,we found the interchange between grooming and infant handling to be bidirectional and to change with the age and number of infants.The interchange relationship between grooming and infant handling is an important part of social relationship maintenance and group stability in primate societies.It not only helps us to understand the characteristics of behavioral adaptation in adult individuals,but also highlights the social functions of infants.

    COMPETING INTERESTS

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

    AUTHORS’CONTRIBUTIONS

    Q.J.and D.P.X.designed the study. Q.J.and K.H.P.conducted data collection.Q.J.and K.H.P.performed statistical analysis,Q.J.and D.P.X.wrote the manuscript,and J.H.L.revised the manuscript.All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

    ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

    This study was conducted at Mt.Huangshan China.We give special thanks to Mr.HB Cheng and family for their outstanding logistical support during our study.We are also very grateful to Mr.SF Xie,YG Xie and the staff of Huangshan Monkey Center for their complete support and assistance.

    99久久精品国产亚洲精品| 中文字幕av电影在线播放| 超碰成人久久| 神马国产精品三级电影在线观看 | 少妇被粗大的猛进出69影院| 18禁美女被吸乳视频| 国产国语露脸激情在线看| 国产高清videossex| 搡老熟女国产l中国老女人| 免费在线观看影片大全网站| 久久香蕉国产精品| 日韩大码丰满熟妇| 制服人妻中文乱码| 久久香蕉精品热| 一区二区日韩欧美中文字幕| 一边摸一边抽搐一进一小说| 久久人妻av系列| 日本熟妇午夜| 久久人人精品亚洲av| 国产三级黄色录像| 成年人黄色毛片网站| aaaaa片日本免费| www.www免费av| 精品久久久久久,| 日韩欧美 国产精品| 精品国产亚洲在线| 国产一级毛片七仙女欲春2 | 91av网站免费观看| 精品久久久久久久末码| 亚洲成av人片免费观看| 无人区码免费观看不卡| 精品日产1卡2卡| 伦理电影免费视频| 久久青草综合色| 国产熟女xx| 搞女人的毛片| 亚洲人成网站在线播放欧美日韩| 久久久久久人人人人人| 亚洲欧美日韩无卡精品| 国产免费av片在线观看野外av| 国产三级黄色录像| 亚洲黑人精品在线| 成熟少妇高潮喷水视频| www日本在线高清视频| 久久人人精品亚洲av| 亚洲性夜色夜夜综合| 日韩一卡2卡3卡4卡2021年| 精品高清国产在线一区| 国产av在哪里看| 亚洲av电影在线进入| 无人区码免费观看不卡| 熟妇人妻久久中文字幕3abv| av欧美777| 啪啪无遮挡十八禁网站| 久久热在线av| 久久精品人妻少妇| 欧美激情高清一区二区三区| 国产精品久久电影中文字幕| 黑人巨大精品欧美一区二区mp4| 黄色女人牲交| 后天国语完整版免费观看| 免费观看精品视频网站| 亚洲精品一区av在线观看| 成在线人永久免费视频| 色综合欧美亚洲国产小说| 免费在线观看视频国产中文字幕亚洲| 久久亚洲精品不卡| 成人手机av| 人人澡人人妻人| 身体一侧抽搐| 啦啦啦 在线观看视频| 一级作爱视频免费观看| 欧美乱码精品一区二区三区| 国产成人系列免费观看| 在线观看午夜福利视频| 日韩大尺度精品在线看网址| 欧美日本亚洲视频在线播放| 一级片免费观看大全| 国产精品 欧美亚洲| 亚洲欧美精品综合一区二区三区| 亚洲精品美女久久av网站| 国产熟女午夜一区二区三区| 成人欧美大片| www.自偷自拍.com| 男女之事视频高清在线观看| 波多野结衣高清无吗| 国产精品一区二区三区四区久久 | 成人精品一区二区免费| 18禁观看日本| 免费观看人在逋| 欧美中文日本在线观看视频| 精品福利观看| 岛国视频午夜一区免费看| 亚洲成av人片免费观看| 国产亚洲精品一区二区www| 村上凉子中文字幕在线| 俄罗斯特黄特色一大片| 伦理电影免费视频| 男人舔奶头视频| 国产精品自产拍在线观看55亚洲| 可以免费在线观看a视频的电影网站| 超碰成人久久| 美女高潮喷水抽搐中文字幕| 免费在线观看亚洲国产| 美女国产高潮福利片在线看| 欧美黄色片欧美黄色片| 日本撒尿小便嘘嘘汇集6| 国产精品亚洲美女久久久| 亚洲第一av免费看| 一本一本综合久久| 老司机深夜福利视频在线观看| 精华霜和精华液先用哪个| 久久亚洲真实| 久久人妻av系列| 国内少妇人妻偷人精品xxx网站 | av有码第一页| 很黄的视频免费| 免费在线观看视频国产中文字幕亚洲| 亚洲av美国av| 夜夜夜夜夜久久久久| 天天一区二区日本电影三级| 88av欧美| 国产97色在线日韩免费| 黑人巨大精品欧美一区二区mp4| 十八禁网站免费在线| 在线观看免费午夜福利视频| 亚洲国产日韩欧美精品在线观看 | 成年女人毛片免费观看观看9| 曰老女人黄片| 男女做爰动态图高潮gif福利片| 国产成人影院久久av| 中文字幕久久专区| 亚洲国产看品久久| 成年人黄色毛片网站| 色综合婷婷激情| 欧美av亚洲av综合av国产av| tocl精华| 美国免费a级毛片| 国产一区在线观看成人免费| 1024手机看黄色片| 97碰自拍视频| 亚洲九九香蕉| 黄色丝袜av网址大全| 妹子高潮喷水视频| 一夜夜www| 亚洲第一电影网av| 国产男靠女视频免费网站| 亚洲欧美一区二区三区黑人| 啦啦啦观看免费观看视频高清| 久久欧美精品欧美久久欧美| 最近最新中文字幕大全免费视频| 精品第一国产精品| 亚洲国产精品999在线| 欧美成人一区二区免费高清观看 | 老司机福利观看| 一区二区三区激情视频| 精品久久久久久久毛片微露脸| 婷婷丁香在线五月| 黄网站色视频无遮挡免费观看| 欧美性猛交黑人性爽| 精品久久蜜臀av无| 国产爱豆传媒在线观看 | 99久久综合精品五月天人人| 国产成人欧美| 免费人成视频x8x8入口观看| 一级毛片精品| 国产高清激情床上av| 久久午夜亚洲精品久久| 女人爽到高潮嗷嗷叫在线视频| 中文字幕人成人乱码亚洲影| 在线观看免费日韩欧美大片| 久久精品国产综合久久久| 色在线成人网| 一本一本综合久久| 精品福利观看| 亚洲狠狠婷婷综合久久图片| 99热6这里只有精品| 天天一区二区日本电影三级| 久久久水蜜桃国产精品网| 久久久久久亚洲精品国产蜜桃av| 动漫黄色视频在线观看| 久久精品国产亚洲av高清一级| 国产在线精品亚洲第一网站| 亚洲自拍偷在线| 桃红色精品国产亚洲av| 欧美日韩瑟瑟在线播放| 亚洲熟妇熟女久久| 国产成人啪精品午夜网站| 欧美三级亚洲精品| 亚洲精品中文字幕一二三四区| 精品第一国产精品| 人人妻人人澡欧美一区二区| 欧美色视频一区免费| 无限看片的www在线观看| 国产精品免费一区二区三区在线| 91麻豆av在线| 日韩欧美国产一区二区入口| 禁无遮挡网站| 又黄又粗又硬又大视频| 女生性感内裤真人,穿戴方法视频| 两人在一起打扑克的视频| 亚洲精品一卡2卡三卡4卡5卡| 欧美激情极品国产一区二区三区| 国产av一区在线观看免费| 天天躁夜夜躁狠狠躁躁| 男人的好看免费观看在线视频 | 女性被躁到高潮视频| 国产精品99久久99久久久不卡| 伦理电影免费视频| 亚洲自偷自拍图片 自拍| 男女午夜视频在线观看| 亚洲中文av在线| 精品欧美国产一区二区三| 色尼玛亚洲综合影院| 欧美日韩精品网址| 一区福利在线观看| 丝袜美腿诱惑在线| 国产视频内射| 99国产精品99久久久久| 亚洲精品中文字幕在线视频| 午夜福利成人在线免费观看| 欧美性长视频在线观看| 啦啦啦免费观看视频1| 日韩欧美免费精品| 国产精品亚洲av一区麻豆| 成人永久免费在线观看视频| 免费av毛片视频| xxxwww97欧美| 一进一出抽搐gif免费好疼| 日韩av在线大香蕉| 在线观看舔阴道视频| 国产精品,欧美在线| 三级毛片av免费| 麻豆成人av在线观看| 亚洲av日韩精品久久久久久密| 日本在线视频免费播放| 国产在线观看jvid| 日韩一卡2卡3卡4卡2021年| 成人特级黄色片久久久久久久| 性色av乱码一区二区三区2| 淫妇啪啪啪对白视频| 亚洲第一av免费看| 国内精品久久久久久久电影| 亚洲一区二区三区色噜噜| 亚洲欧美一区二区三区黑人| 久久久久免费精品人妻一区二区 | 久久亚洲真实| 熟妇人妻久久中文字幕3abv| 变态另类丝袜制服| 欧美日韩亚洲国产一区二区在线观看| 日韩精品青青久久久久久| 日韩高清综合在线| 成人国产综合亚洲| 亚洲av电影在线进入| 制服人妻中文乱码| 国产蜜桃级精品一区二区三区| e午夜精品久久久久久久| 亚洲 欧美 日韩 在线 免费| 啪啪无遮挡十八禁网站| 天天一区二区日本电影三级| 精品国产乱码久久久久久男人| 高潮久久久久久久久久久不卡| 在线观看一区二区三区| 亚洲一区高清亚洲精品| 男人舔奶头视频| 大香蕉久久成人网| 岛国在线观看网站| 亚洲av电影在线进入| 精品久久久久久久久久免费视频| 国产免费av片在线观看野外av| 国产区一区二久久| 亚洲 国产 在线| 一区二区三区精品91| 色婷婷久久久亚洲欧美| 久久人妻福利社区极品人妻图片| 久久久久久久久中文| 18禁国产床啪视频网站| 亚洲男人的天堂狠狠| av天堂在线播放| 国产av又大| 国产精品一区二区精品视频观看| 免费女性裸体啪啪无遮挡网站| 啦啦啦观看免费观看视频高清| 成人三级黄色视频| 桃色一区二区三区在线观看| 亚洲专区中文字幕在线| 99在线视频只有这里精品首页| 欧美日韩精品网址| 国产成人精品久久二区二区91| 人人妻人人看人人澡| 男女床上黄色一级片免费看| 国产又黄又爽又无遮挡在线| 91麻豆精品激情在线观看国产| 欧美丝袜亚洲另类 | 激情在线观看视频在线高清| 欧美日韩乱码在线| 欧美日本视频| 91成年电影在线观看| 亚洲天堂国产精品一区在线| 国产主播在线观看一区二区| 欧美另类亚洲清纯唯美| 亚洲自拍偷在线| 免费在线观看黄色视频的| 亚洲熟女毛片儿| 99久久无色码亚洲精品果冻| netflix在线观看网站| 日本精品一区二区三区蜜桃| 久久热在线av| 亚洲第一青青草原| 国产aⅴ精品一区二区三区波| 日韩精品免费视频一区二区三区| 狂野欧美激情性xxxx| 自线自在国产av| 欧美乱妇无乱码| 久热爱精品视频在线9| 精品免费久久久久久久清纯| 国产伦一二天堂av在线观看| 91成年电影在线观看| 国内精品久久久久精免费| 国产主播在线观看一区二区| 国产激情久久老熟女| 亚洲国产毛片av蜜桃av| 十分钟在线观看高清视频www| 亚洲成人久久性| 这个男人来自地球电影免费观看| 变态另类成人亚洲欧美熟女| 一边摸一边抽搐一进一小说| 日本成人三级电影网站| www.www免费av| av免费在线观看网站| 精品免费久久久久久久清纯| 亚洲最大成人中文| 51午夜福利影视在线观看| 中文资源天堂在线| 听说在线观看完整版免费高清| xxx96com| 91成人精品电影| 母亲3免费完整高清在线观看| 国产成人av激情在线播放| 嫩草影视91久久| a级毛片a级免费在线| 色av中文字幕| 自线自在国产av| 国产色视频综合| 亚洲午夜精品一区,二区,三区| 国产av不卡久久| 成年女人毛片免费观看观看9| 亚洲中文字幕一区二区三区有码在线看 | 欧美午夜高清在线| 久久久久国内视频| 91在线观看av| 亚洲激情在线av| 狂野欧美激情性xxxx| 999久久久国产精品视频| 日本免费a在线| 淫妇啪啪啪对白视频| www日本黄色视频网| 1024手机看黄色片| 国产精品乱码一区二三区的特点| 久久久久久国产a免费观看| 国产久久久一区二区三区| 午夜福利高清视频| 一个人免费在线观看的高清视频| 国产成人欧美在线观看| 精品国产亚洲在线| 久久久精品欧美日韩精品| 欧洲精品卡2卡3卡4卡5卡区| 国产一级毛片七仙女欲春2 | 欧美久久黑人一区二区| 99国产极品粉嫩在线观看| а√天堂www在线а√下载| 欧美日韩乱码在线| 人妻丰满熟妇av一区二区三区| 国内揄拍国产精品人妻在线 | 国产精品永久免费网站| 波多野结衣高清无吗| 欧美成人一区二区免费高清观看 | 成年版毛片免费区| 少妇被粗大的猛进出69影院| 精品欧美国产一区二区三| 成人18禁高潮啪啪吃奶动态图| 美女午夜性视频免费| 久久久国产成人免费| 女性被躁到高潮视频| 一二三四社区在线视频社区8| 亚洲av电影不卡..在线观看| 国内少妇人妻偷人精品xxx网站 | 一区二区三区激情视频| 51午夜福利影视在线观看| 中文字幕久久专区| 一区二区日韩欧美中文字幕| 精品无人区乱码1区二区| 国产视频一区二区在线看| 国产亚洲精品久久久久5区| 50天的宝宝边吃奶边哭怎么回事| 午夜影院日韩av| 麻豆av在线久日| 国产亚洲精品综合一区在线观看 | 久久精品影院6| 精品日产1卡2卡| 亚洲中文字幕一区二区三区有码在线看 | 精品第一国产精品| 欧美日本亚洲视频在线播放| 母亲3免费完整高清在线观看| 久久国产亚洲av麻豆专区| 美女午夜性视频免费| 中文字幕人妻熟女乱码| 国产精品98久久久久久宅男小说| 国产一区二区激情短视频| 午夜成年电影在线免费观看| 国产男靠女视频免费网站| 国产真实乱freesex| 99久久久亚洲精品蜜臀av| 亚洲一卡2卡3卡4卡5卡精品中文| 久久精品人妻少妇| 欧美中文日本在线观看视频| 欧美在线黄色| 法律面前人人平等表现在哪些方面| 国产精品久久久久久精品电影 | 欧美性猛交黑人性爽| 在线观看舔阴道视频| 亚洲欧美一区二区三区黑人| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 母亲3免费完整高清在线观看| 一二三四在线观看免费中文在| 十八禁人妻一区二区| 在线观看www视频免费| 欧美色视频一区免费| 日韩免费av在线播放| 热99re8久久精品国产| 久久久国产欧美日韩av| 欧美日韩亚洲国产一区二区在线观看| 91麻豆精品激情在线观看国产| 精品久久久久久久久久免费视频| 淫秽高清视频在线观看| 韩国av一区二区三区四区| 性色av乱码一区二区三区2| 欧美不卡视频在线免费观看 | 九色国产91popny在线| 亚洲国产欧洲综合997久久, | 一夜夜www| 国语自产精品视频在线第100页| 最近最新免费中文字幕在线| 精品一区二区三区视频在线观看免费| 无人区码免费观看不卡| 成人三级黄色视频| 国产片内射在线| 亚洲精品色激情综合| 中文字幕av电影在线播放| 欧美+亚洲+日韩+国产| 亚洲五月色婷婷综合| 午夜福利免费观看在线| 精品国产乱子伦一区二区三区| 国产一卡二卡三卡精品| 夜夜躁狠狠躁天天躁| 日韩三级视频一区二区三区| 在线观看舔阴道视频| 男女之事视频高清在线观看| 一边摸一边做爽爽视频免费| 精品熟女少妇八av免费久了| 欧美日本亚洲视频在线播放| svipshipincom国产片| 88av欧美| 91九色精品人成在线观看| 18禁美女被吸乳视频| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 色播亚洲综合网| 日韩国内少妇激情av| 国产乱人伦免费视频| 老汉色av国产亚洲站长工具| 伦理电影免费视频| 国产在线观看jvid| 国产精品美女特级片免费视频播放器 | 身体一侧抽搐| 1024视频免费在线观看| 熟妇人妻久久中文字幕3abv| 女性生殖器流出的白浆| 中文在线观看免费www的网站 | 一进一出好大好爽视频| 国产单亲对白刺激| 精品人妻1区二区| 亚洲人成电影免费在线| 国产一级毛片七仙女欲春2 | 久久香蕉精品热| 老鸭窝网址在线观看| 岛国在线观看网站| 性色av乱码一区二区三区2| 99久久99久久久精品蜜桃| 欧美激情极品国产一区二区三区| 国内精品久久久久精免费| 亚洲精品美女久久久久99蜜臀| 久久国产精品人妻蜜桃| 欧美午夜高清在线| 级片在线观看| 国产一卡二卡三卡精品| 精品第一国产精品| 亚洲成av片中文字幕在线观看| 国产精品久久电影中文字幕| 日韩欧美一区二区三区在线观看| 国产成人一区二区三区免费视频网站| 欧美一区二区精品小视频在线| 可以在线观看的亚洲视频| 人人妻人人澡欧美一区二区| www.熟女人妻精品国产| 亚洲熟妇熟女久久| 在线十欧美十亚洲十日本专区| 18美女黄网站色大片免费观看| 午夜福利高清视频| 欧美日韩亚洲国产一区二区在线观看| 一二三四社区在线视频社区8| 国产熟女xx| 久久精品91无色码中文字幕| 18禁观看日本| 日韩欧美在线二视频| 久久精品影院6| 中文字幕精品免费在线观看视频| 大香蕉久久成人网| 2021天堂中文幕一二区在线观 | 国产精品日韩av在线免费观看| www日本在线高清视频| 午夜福利视频1000在线观看| 人人妻,人人澡人人爽秒播| 日本三级黄在线观看| 变态另类丝袜制服| 美女国产高潮福利片在线看| 在线观看免费午夜福利视频| 国内揄拍国产精品人妻在线 | 日本精品一区二区三区蜜桃| 熟妇人妻久久中文字幕3abv| 两性夫妻黄色片| 亚洲国产精品sss在线观看| 91大片在线观看| 一个人免费在线观看的高清视频| 久热爱精品视频在线9| 国产成人av教育| 久久狼人影院| 成人欧美大片| 久久精品国产亚洲av高清一级| 精品国产超薄肉色丝袜足j| 国产精品亚洲av一区麻豆| 欧美黑人欧美精品刺激| 成人永久免费在线观看视频| 大香蕉久久成人网| 欧美国产日韩亚洲一区| 国产午夜精品久久久久久| 岛国在线观看网站| 午夜精品久久久久久毛片777| 国产又爽黄色视频| 国产99久久九九免费精品| av视频在线观看入口| 妹子高潮喷水视频| 91麻豆av在线| 久久久水蜜桃国产精品网| 欧美国产精品va在线观看不卡| 欧美人与性动交α欧美精品济南到| 国产一区在线观看成人免费| 日本在线视频免费播放| 国产私拍福利视频在线观看| 亚洲精品av麻豆狂野| 成在线人永久免费视频| 无限看片的www在线观看| 国产精品爽爽va在线观看网站 | 伊人久久大香线蕉亚洲五| 国产成人av教育| 精品人妻1区二区| 亚洲欧美精品综合久久99| 国产主播在线观看一区二区| 级片在线观看| 天堂动漫精品| 国产亚洲精品综合一区在线观看 | 最近最新中文字幕大全免费视频| 亚洲午夜精品一区,二区,三区| a级毛片在线看网站| 国产视频一区二区在线看| 久久久国产成人免费| 国产精品影院久久| 久久久国产成人精品二区| 久久国产精品男人的天堂亚洲| 日韩欧美在线二视频| 国产精品,欧美在线| 免费观看精品视频网站| 亚洲无线在线观看| 久久久久免费精品人妻一区二区 | 亚洲男人的天堂狠狠| 18禁国产床啪视频网站| 人人澡人人妻人| 亚洲人成77777在线视频| 熟妇人妻久久中文字幕3abv| 色播亚洲综合网| 国产视频一区二区在线看| 久久香蕉激情| 日韩欧美国产一区二区入口| 99久久综合精品五月天人人| 色综合亚洲欧美另类图片| netflix在线观看网站| 国产精华一区二区三区| 亚洲第一av免费看| 精品高清国产在线一区| 高清毛片免费观看视频网站| 亚洲国产精品999在线| 久久国产亚洲av麻豆专区| 视频在线观看一区二区三区| 亚洲美女黄片视频| 国产激情欧美一区二区| 国产精品久久视频播放| 国产av在哪里看| 看免费av毛片| 亚洲五月天丁香| 精品久久久久久,| 一级毛片精品| 免费看十八禁软件| 波多野结衣av一区二区av| 成人欧美大片| 在线观看午夜福利视频| 一级a爱片免费观看的视频|