郝亞楠 宋勃東 王 巖 張 欽 郎 越
(首都師范大學(xué)心理學(xué)系, 北京市“學(xué)習(xí)與認(rèn)知”重點(diǎn)實(shí)驗(yàn)室, 北京 100037)
詩人拜倫曾說過, “悲觀的人雖生猶死, 樂觀的人永生不老”。積極心理學(xué)之父Martin Seligman也曾在他的暢銷書《活出最樂觀的自己》中談到,樂觀帶給人們大量的好處(洪蘭譯, Seligman,2010/1991), 可以體現(xiàn)在身心健康, 社會適應(yīng)等諸多方面。這與大量學(xué)者通過實(shí)證研究考察樂觀對個(gè)體身體、心理和行為等方面影響的結(jié)果相一致。比如, 樂觀與身體健康(Hingle et al., 2014;Warner, Schwarzer, Schüz, Wurm, & Tesch-R?mer,2012)、幸福感(Solberg Nes, Evans, & Segerstrom,2009; Ammirati, Lamis, Campos, & Farber, 2015)、生活質(zhì)量(Wrosch & Scheier, 2003; Zaslavsky et al.,2015)和工作滿意度(Kwok, Cheng, & Wong, 2015)等方面的提高存在顯著正相關(guān)。樂觀還與抗壓能力的增強(qiáng)(Jobin, Wrosch, & Scheier, 2014; Taylor,Larsen-Rife, Conger, Widaman, & Cutrona, 2010),抑郁癥狀的緩解(Hirsch, Nsamenang, Chang, &Kaslow, 2014)、創(chuàng)傷后應(yīng)激障礙綜合征的減少(Gil& Weinberg, 2015), 以及創(chuàng)傷后的成長(Shand,Cowlishaw, Brooker, Burney, & Ricciardelli, 2015)等存在顯著正相關(guān)。不過, 樂觀的前期研究大都集中在它與其他主題的關(guān)系方面, 主要描述樂觀者的行為表現(xiàn)。但是有關(guān)樂觀心理機(jī)制探討的研究較少, 僅有Aspinwal, Richter和Hoffman (2001)提出的樂觀信念的適應(yīng)功能(the adaptiveness of optimistic beliefs), 即樂觀的信念有助于個(gè)體靈活地分配認(rèn)知資源, 從而更好地追求目標(biāo)。但是該理論的后續(xù)研究很少。而近年, 隨著積極心理學(xué)的興起, 越來越多的研究者開始深入探討樂觀影響個(gè)體行為的內(nèi)在心理機(jī)制, 這對于解釋樂觀的發(fā)生具有重要的啟示意義。
人格維度的樂觀和悲觀廣泛存在于大眾的智慧中, 而將樂觀作為一種人格特質(zhì)的研究, 可追溯到30年前樂觀問卷——生活定向測驗(yàn)(the Life Orientation Test, 簡稱 LOT) ——的誕生(Scheier& Carver, 1985)。在該研究中, Scheier和 Carver界定了氣質(zhì)性樂觀(dispositional optimism)即一種對未來的總體的積極期待。這一定義提出的依據(jù)是動機(jī)的自我調(diào)節(jié)理論——期待?價(jià)值理論(Scheier & Carver, 1992)。該理論強(qiáng)調(diào)期待在行為調(diào)節(jié)中的關(guān)鍵作用, 當(dāng)個(gè)體對目標(biāo)持有積極期待時(shí), 他們會采取趨近行為不斷縮短自己與目標(biāo)的距離直至實(shí)現(xiàn)目標(biāo); 當(dāng)個(gè)體對目標(biāo)持有消極期待時(shí), 他們會采取回避行為并放棄目標(biāo)(Scheier &Carver, 1985, 1992), 這就是樂觀(optimism)和悲觀(pessimism)。樂觀既是一種穩(wěn)定的人格特質(zhì),又是一種認(rèn)知結(jié)構(gòu), 其核心是期待。它表現(xiàn)為期待未來有很多好事情的發(fā)生(Scheier & Carver,1992; Carver & Scheier, 2014)。因此, 在面對困難時(shí), 氣質(zhì)性樂觀者傾向于堅(jiān)信自己的目標(biāo)能夠?qū)崿F(xiàn), 受趨近動機(jī)調(diào)節(jié)和支配; 氣質(zhì)性悲觀者則傾向于放棄目標(biāo), 受回避動機(jī)調(diào)節(jié)和支配(De Pascalis, Cozzuto, Caprara, & Alessandri, 2013;Rygula, Golebiowska, Kregiel, Kubik, & Popik,2015)。另外, Peterson和Seligman (2004)還將樂觀作為24項(xiàng)性格優(yōu)勢之一, 認(rèn)為樂觀與希望、未來意識(future-mindedness)及未來定向(future orientation)一起代表了指向未來的認(rèn)知、情緒與動機(jī)心理層面的積極特質(zhì)。
氣質(zhì)性樂觀的常用測量工具是生活定向測驗(yàn)(LOT) (Scheier & Carver, 1985), 它于1994年得到修訂(LOT-R)。該測驗(yàn)(LOT-R)采取自我報(bào)告法,共10個(gè)題目, 包括6個(gè)正式題目和4個(gè)附加題目,得分越高代表越樂觀。它測量了樂觀和悲觀兩個(gè)維度, 具有較高的信效度(Scheier, Carver, & Bridges,1994; Steca, Monzani, Greco, Chiesi, & Primi, 2015;溫娟娟, 2012), 而且被翻譯修訂成如漢語(溫娟娟,2012), 葡萄牙語(Laranjeira, 2008), 德語(Glaesmer,Hoyer, Klotsche, & Herzberg, 2008), 希伯來語(Benyamini & Raz, 2007)等多種語言, 是目前樂觀研究中使用最多的問卷。
當(dāng)前, 盡管學(xué)者對樂觀概念的界定有很多種,如樂觀的解釋風(fēng)格(洪蘭譯, Seligman, 2010/1991)、學(xué)業(yè)樂觀(Beard, Hoy, & Woolfolk Hoy, 2010;Sezgin & Erdogan, 2015)等等, 但是因?yàn)長OT-R問卷的廣泛應(yīng)用, 使得其所測量的氣質(zhì)性樂觀成為目前樂觀研究中最常用的界定方法。本文中的樂觀專指氣質(zhì)性樂觀。
為了進(jìn)一步明確樂觀概念的界定, 我們將其與心理學(xué)中最相近的概念——希望——進(jìn)行比較。Snyder等人(1991), Snyder, Feldman, Taylor,Schroeder和 Adams (2000)將希望(hope)定義為一種積極的動機(jī)性狀態(tài), 這種狀態(tài)是以指向目標(biāo)的動力和達(dá)到目標(biāo)的路徑的交互作用為基礎(chǔ)形成的(引劉孟超, 黃希庭, 2013), 也就是說希望是動機(jī)和方法的交互作用。樂觀和希望均是一種指向未來的人格特質(zhì)。而且, 一些研究者用希望和樂觀的測量工具(AHS/HS vs. LOT-R)對兩者的關(guān)系進(jìn)行實(shí)證研究, 發(fā)現(xiàn)兩者之間存在顯著正相關(guān)(Rajandram et al., 2011; Rand, 2009)。
雖然樂觀和希望存在共同點(diǎn), 但它們并非同一概念。從定義來說, 樂觀更強(qiáng)調(diào)達(dá)到目標(biāo)的動力的重要性, 即對未來的積極期待會促使人們行動, 但并未明確說明如何行動。而希望則將動力和路線放到了同等重要的位置(Snyder et al., 2000;Snyder, 2002)。從實(shí)證研究來看, 有研究者對1996至 2012年發(fā)表的希望和樂觀的文獻(xiàn)進(jìn)行元分析,發(fā)現(xiàn)樂觀與某些變量諸如自尊或正性負(fù)性情緒等的相關(guān)顯著大于希望, 而希望與壓力、幸福感的相關(guān)顯著大于樂觀(Alarcon, Bowling, & Khazon,2013)。還有研究發(fā)現(xiàn), 在預(yù)測應(yīng)對策略如積極重評的使用中, 樂觀的影響比希望大。反之, 在預(yù)測自我效能感時(shí), 希望的影響顯著大于樂觀。此外,該研究還發(fā)現(xiàn)相對于希望的目標(biāo), 樂觀的目標(biāo)更為寬泛(Bryant & Cvengros, 2004)。可以看出, 樂觀與希望是有一定聯(lián)系但卻不同的兩個(gè)概念。
樂觀者總是在每一個(gè)困難中看到機(jī)會, 悲觀者總是在每一個(gè)機(jī)會中看到困難。溫斯頓?丘吉爾的這句名言, 是樂觀者具有正性偏向的最好闡述。研究也發(fā)現(xiàn), 活得開心幸福的人似乎帶著玫瑰色的眼鏡, 更愿意對積極刺激多加注意(Raila,Scholl, & Gruber, 2015)。從樂觀的定義來看, 樂觀者持有積極期待并且擁有趨近動機(jī)。而他們所具有的這一正性偏向不僅體現(xiàn)在期待和動機(jī)方面,還體現(xiàn)在注意、知覺、記憶、解釋風(fēng)格、應(yīng)對策略的選擇及情緒方面。這些既是樂觀者的行為表現(xiàn), 也是樂觀者的心理反應(yīng)。
注意偏向(attentional bias)是指相對中性刺激,個(gè)體對情緒刺激表現(xiàn)出不同的注意分配(Cisler &Koster, 2010)。樂觀者表現(xiàn)出對正性信息的注意偏向。Segerstrom (2001)要求被試判斷正、負(fù)性情緒詞、工具詞匯(控制條件)的書寫顏色, 發(fā)現(xiàn)樂觀者判斷正性情緒詞產(chǎn)生的Stroop效應(yīng)大于負(fù)性情緒詞; 悲觀者僅在判斷負(fù)性情緒詞時(shí)產(chǎn)生Stroop效應(yīng), 判斷正性情緒詞產(chǎn)生的 Stroop效應(yīng)不顯著。這說明, 詞的情緒性信息有效地干擾了不同樂觀程度被試對詞顏色的命名, 樂觀者產(chǎn)生了對正性情緒詞的注意偏向, 悲觀者則對負(fù)性情緒詞產(chǎn)生了注意偏向。同樣采用情緒 Stroop范式, 另有研究得出了相似的結(jié)果即樂觀者命名正性情緒詞顏色的時(shí)間顯著長于中性詞(Karademas, Kafetsios,& Sideridis, 2007)。同時(shí), 樂觀者對負(fù)性信息的注意會減少。比如, Isaacowitz (2005)運(yùn)用眼動追蹤技術(shù)考察樂觀者的注意偏向, 實(shí)驗(yàn)要求被試觀察皮膚癌圖片, 結(jié)果表明與悲觀者相比, 樂觀者會選擇性地忽視負(fù)性信息, 這從另一側(cè)面體現(xiàn)了樂觀者的正性偏向。
知覺偏向(bias in perception)是指由于知覺主客體的特殊性以及一些外界環(huán)境因素的作用, 人在知覺過程中會產(chǎn)生一些與客觀現(xiàn)實(shí)不相符合的知覺結(jié)論(周曉虹, 1997)。樂觀者表現(xiàn)出對正性信息的知覺偏向。例如在遭遇相同困難時(shí), 樂觀者報(bào)告獲得的社會支持高于悲觀者(Srivastava,McGonigal, Richards, Butler, & Gross, 2006;Wimberly, Carver, & Antoni, 2008)。而且在控制了社會支持的實(shí)際給予量后, 他們知覺到的依然較高 (Vollmann, Antoniw, Hartung, & Renner, 2011)。另外, 樂觀者在追求目標(biāo)過程中報(bào)告獲得的進(jìn)步要比悲觀者多 (Monzani et al, 2015)。相應(yīng)地, 樂觀者知覺到負(fù)性信息就會減少。如面對同樣的負(fù)性事件, 樂觀者知覺到的壓力強(qiáng)度比悲觀者小(Jobin et al., 2014)。同樣, 相對悲觀者, 樂觀者知覺到的疼痛較輕(Geers, Wellman, Helfer, Fowler,& France, 2008; Hood, Pulvers, Carillo, Merchant,& Thomas, 2012)且持續(xù)時(shí)間也較短(Goodin,Kronfli, et al., 2013)。
記憶偏向(memory bias)指在控制了一般記憶能力后, 某種人格特質(zhì)差異對某一特殊類型經(jīng)驗(yàn)的回憶或再認(rèn)有更好或更壞的傾向(引楊慧, 吳明證, 劉永芳, Li, 2012)。樂觀者表現(xiàn)出對正性信息的記憶偏向, 也就是說, 樂觀者更容易回憶起好的、正性的或有利于自己的內(nèi)容。例如在玩撲克牌二十一點(diǎn)游戲時(shí), 盡管成功的概率是隨機(jī)的,但是樂觀者一周后回憶贏錢的次數(shù)顯著多于悲觀者。換句話說, 樂觀者回憶出的正性信息比悲觀者多(Gibson & Sanbonmatsu, 2004)。還有研究要求被試瀏覽甲狀腺機(jī)能亢進(jìn)相關(guān)的負(fù)性、中性及正性信息后進(jìn)行自由回憶, 發(fā)現(xiàn)氣質(zhì)性樂觀分?jǐn)?shù)越高的被試回憶出正性信息的數(shù)量越多(Luo,2008)。同樣采用自由回憶的方式, 另有研究也發(fā)現(xiàn), 樂觀者回憶出積極詞匯的數(shù)量多于悲觀者(李燚, 2011)。
解釋風(fēng)格是一種認(rèn)知性人格變量, 是人們對發(fā)生在自己身上不好事件的原因進(jìn)行解釋時(shí)的習(xí)慣性態(tài)度(Peterson & Seligman, 1984)。氣質(zhì)性樂觀者表現(xiàn)出積極的解釋風(fēng)格即他們會將壞事情歸因?yàn)椴环€(wěn)定的、特定的、外在的。Peterson (2000)曾提出大樂觀(氣質(zhì)性樂觀)和小樂觀(樂觀的解釋風(fēng)格)的概念, 并指出兩者相關(guān)的必然性。國內(nèi)溫娟娟、鄭雪和張靈(2007)也認(rèn)為樂觀是一種人格特質(zhì), 表現(xiàn)為一種積極的解釋風(fēng)格。而且, 從理論上來說, 氣質(zhì)性樂觀作為一種人格特質(zhì), 可能會影響人們的解釋風(fēng)格(Rusting & Larsen, 1998), 即樂觀者在面對挫折時(shí)傾向于采取積極的解釋風(fēng)格。從實(shí)證研究來說, 已有研究也發(fā)現(xiàn)氣質(zhì)性樂觀和樂觀的解釋風(fēng)格存在顯著正相關(guān)(溫娟娟, 鄭雪,2011; 祁珍華, 陳瑨, 江虹, 張紅靜, 2012; Zhang et al., 2014)。
不同的解釋風(fēng)格可能是造成樂觀者和悲觀者行為結(jié)果差異的重要原因。如在追求成功方面,具有樂觀解釋風(fēng)格的人在遇到困難時(shí)更容易繼續(xù)堅(jiān)持, 這不僅體現(xiàn)在保險(xiǎn)銷售方面(Seligman &Schulman, 1986), 還表現(xiàn)在軍事院校的訓(xùn)練(Schulman, Castellon, & Seligman, 1989)或者團(tuán)體運(yùn)動方面(Carron, Shapcott, & Martin, 2014); 在學(xué)業(yè)方面, 采取樂觀解釋風(fēng)格的學(xué)生的學(xué)業(yè)成績高于那些持有悲觀解釋風(fēng)格的學(xué)生(Maleva, Westcott,McKellop, McLaughlin, & Widman, 2014; Peterson& Barrett, 1987)。而在其他方面諸如面對負(fù)性事件時(shí), 持有樂觀解釋風(fēng)格的學(xué)生的自殺意圖也少于悲觀的學(xué)生(Hirsch & Rabon, 2015)。
應(yīng)對策略是指個(gè)體面對不同的應(yīng)激事件時(shí)所采取的認(rèn)知和行為方式(Lazarus & Folkman,1984)。它和上述解釋風(fēng)格既有區(qū)別又有聯(lián)系, 解釋風(fēng)格是對人們行為的原因進(jìn)行歸因, 而應(yīng)對策略是個(gè)體對壓力性事件或情境的威脅性進(jìn)行評估并作出相應(yīng)的行為反應(yīng)。解釋風(fēng)格會影響人們對事件的應(yīng)對策略, 對事件的積極解釋有利于個(gè)體采取積極應(yīng)對策略, 對事件的消極解釋則有利于個(gè)體采取消極的應(yīng)對策略(Sanjuan & Magallares,2015)。樂觀者持有積極解釋風(fēng)格, 面對壓力事件傾向于采取積極應(yīng)對策略。已有研究發(fā)現(xiàn)樂觀與積極應(yīng)對策略的選擇存在顯著正相關(guān)(Chang &Chan, 2015; Goodin, Glover, et al., 2013; Goodarzi,Shokri, & Sharifi, 2015; Lee & Mason, 2013; Perera& McIlveen, 2014)。另外, Carver等人(1993)對乳腺癌患者進(jìn)行追蹤研究也發(fā)現(xiàn), 樂觀者多采取事先計(jì)劃、積極重塑及幽默等積極應(yīng)對策略; 反之,悲觀者則是采取否定事實(shí)、抑制抵觸情緒及放棄努力等消極應(yīng)對策略。
積極應(yīng)對策略的選擇是使樂觀者獲得更多益處的重要原因。有研究發(fā)現(xiàn)積極應(yīng)對策略的應(yīng)用是使樂觀者比悲觀者更易適應(yīng)壓力的中介變量(Büyüka?ik-?olak, Gündo?du-Aktürk, & Bozo, 2012;Taylor et al., 2010, 2012), 也是減輕疼痛 (Hood et al., 2012; Wright et al., 2011)的中介變量, 還是學(xué)生運(yùn)動員對學(xué)業(yè)和運(yùn)動都感到較高滿意的原因(Gaudreau, Gunnell, Hoar, Thompson, & Lelièvre,2015)。
樂觀者為什么會采取積極應(yīng)對策略?除了與解釋風(fēng)格有關(guān)之外, 還可能與動機(jī)有關(guān)。有研究指出自我決定動機(jī)在樂觀和積極應(yīng)對策略關(guān)系中起中介作用。換言之, 與低樂觀水平的學(xué)生相比,高樂觀水平的學(xué)生在參與活動時(shí)更易被自我決定動機(jī)驅(qū)使, 進(jìn)而采取積極的應(yīng)對策略(Thompson& Gaudreau, 2008)。
樂觀可能使人們長期處于一種積極的心境即積極的情緒狀態(tài)。樂觀者相信未來會發(fā)生很多好事情, 而這些好事情會使人們產(chǎn)生積極情緒(Strümpfer, 2006)。而且, 樂觀者會不斷為預(yù)期目標(biāo)的實(shí)現(xiàn)而努力, 他們在不斷趨近目標(biāo)的過程中也會體驗(yàn)到較多的積極情緒(Carver & Scheier,2001; Ilies & Judge, 2005)。大量實(shí)證研究也發(fā)現(xiàn)樂觀與積極情緒存在顯著正相關(guān)(Cristea et al.,2011; Lench, 2011; Penedo et al., 2003; Segerstrom,Taylor, Kemeny, & Fahey, 1998)。還有研究發(fā)現(xiàn),作為一種樂觀干預(yù)的樂觀訓(xùn)練既提高了樂觀水平也提高了被試的積極情緒體驗(yàn)(Hanssen, Peters,Vlaeyen, Meevissen, & Vancleef, 2013; Peters, Flink,Boersma, & Linton, 2010)。
同時(shí), 樂觀者體驗(yàn)到的消極情緒會少于悲觀者。比如樂觀者觀看悲傷情節(jié)時(shí)產(chǎn)生的負(fù)面認(rèn)知少于悲觀者(McHale, Clark, & Tramonte, 2015)。樂觀者面對心愛之人死亡時(shí)所產(chǎn)生的痛苦體驗(yàn)也比悲觀者少(Boelen, 2015)。此外, 還有研究發(fā)現(xiàn)樂觀與焦慮及抑郁情緒呈顯著負(fù)相關(guān)(李恒芬, 曹素霞, 張勇, 2006; Johnson & Endler, 2002)。
樂觀能夠給個(gè)體帶來諸多好處, 可能是因?yàn)闃酚^者具有正性偏向。這一偏向主要表現(xiàn)在注意、知覺、記憶、解釋風(fēng)格、應(yīng)對策略的選擇、情緒、期待與動機(jī)方面。其中, 期待是樂觀的核心結(jié)構(gòu),而樂觀者持有積極期待可能是他們表現(xiàn)出正性偏向的基礎(chǔ)。期待會影響人們對信息的選擇加工(Iida, Nakao, & Ohira, 2012; Summerfield & Egner,2009)。同理, 積極期待會使樂觀者在認(rèn)知過程中表現(xiàn)出正性偏向。與消極期待和無期待相比, 積極期待會使人更傾向于選擇加工正性信息(McNulty & Karney, 2004; Helfe, Elhai & Geers,2015)。而樂觀者在認(rèn)知過程中表現(xiàn)出的正性偏向會使他們在面對負(fù)性或壓力事件時(shí), 更傾向于采取樂觀的解釋風(fēng)格和積極的應(yīng)對策略。同時(shí), 積極期待也會使樂觀者產(chǎn)生趨近動機(jī)并且誘發(fā)積極情緒(Scheier & Carver, 1985, 1992)。
樂觀者具有正性偏向是樂觀者的心理機(jī)制,但是它并不具有唯一性。另外, 這一偏向可能受其他因素的影響。當(dāng)所呈現(xiàn)的信息具有自我相關(guān)性時(shí), 樂觀者在認(rèn)知過程中并未表現(xiàn)出正性偏向。如有研究發(fā)現(xiàn), 面對自我相關(guān)的正性信息, 樂觀者和悲觀者投入的注意資源并無顯著差異。而對于自我相關(guān)的負(fù)性信息, 樂觀者卻比悲觀者投入更多的注意資源(Aspinwall & Brunhart, 1996)。還有研究發(fā)現(xiàn), 相對于悲觀者, 樂觀者能回憶出更多的自我相關(guān)信息, 但是這與信息的效價(jià)無關(guān)(Abele & Gendolla, 2007)。這或許是因?yàn)榕c自我無關(guān)信息相比, 自我相關(guān)信息具有更重要的意義,所以樂觀者對這些信息進(jìn)行精細(xì)加工以便采取積極主動的應(yīng)對策略。
通過上面的論述, 我們了解到樂觀者比悲觀者關(guān)注更多的正性信息并表現(xiàn)出正性偏向。而究其神經(jīng)機(jī)制可能與前扣帶回喙部(rostral anterior cingulate cortex, rACC)、前額葉皮質(zhì)(prefrontal cortex)的激活水平相關(guān)。在上述這些神經(jīng)機(jī)制方面, 樂觀者和悲觀者表現(xiàn)出顯著的差異。
前扣帶回喙部位于前額葉皮質(zhì)的吻側(cè), 與傳遞情緒與動機(jī)信息的腦區(qū)如杏仁核有緊密聯(lián)系(Vogt & Pandya, 1987)。近年, 有研究發(fā)現(xiàn)與悲觀者相比, 樂觀者前扣帶回喙部的激活水平會增強(qiáng)(Sharot, Riccardi, Raio, & Phelps, 2007)。
Sharot等人(2007)的腦成像研究(functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging, fMRI)發(fā)現(xiàn), 前扣帶回喙部的BOLD (blood oxygenation level dependent)信號差值(想象未來積極事件?想象未來消極事件)與樂觀分?jǐn)?shù)存在顯著正相關(guān)。也就是說, 樂觀分?jǐn)?shù)越高, BOLD的信號差值越大, 即前扣帶回喙部的激活水平在想象未來積極事件時(shí)越高, 在想象未來消極事件時(shí)越低。這說明隨著樂觀分?jǐn)?shù)的升高, 前扣帶回喙部的激活在想象未來積極事件時(shí)會增強(qiáng), 這一增強(qiáng)反映了個(gè)體的自我調(diào)節(jié), 即對正性信息產(chǎn)生注意與警覺偏向。而已有研究發(fā)現(xiàn),前扣帶回喙部激活的增強(qiáng)與正性信息的加工相關(guān)。例如, 有研究采用 fMRI考察時(shí)間估計(jì)任務(wù)中正性反饋與負(fù)性反饋腦區(qū)激活水平的差異, 發(fā)現(xiàn)正性反饋時(shí)前扣帶回喙部的激活水平顯著高于負(fù)性反饋(Nieuwenhuis, Slagter, Von Geusau, Heslenfeld,& Holroyd, 2005)。另外, 在臨床研究方面, 前扣帶回喙部的高激活被認(rèn)為是抗抑郁治療效果的指標(biāo), 即這一激活的增強(qiáng)表示抑郁癥患者癥狀好轉(zhuǎn),比如他們更容易注意正性信息(Hunter, Korb,Cook, & Leuchter, 2013)。這從反面體現(xiàn)了前扣帶回喙部激活水平的增強(qiáng)在正性信息加工中的重要作用。
總之, 隨著樂觀分?jǐn)?shù)的升高, 前扣帶回喙部的激活水平也會增強(qiáng), 而這一區(qū)域激活水平的增強(qiáng)與正性信息的加工相關(guān), 因此, 前扣帶回喙部激活的增強(qiáng)可能是樂觀者對正性信息表現(xiàn)出偏向的原因。
前額葉皮質(zhì)負(fù)責(zé)高級運(yùn)動的控制、計(jì)劃和執(zhí)行。它的解剖結(jié)構(gòu)包括背側(cè)前額葉、扣帶前回、內(nèi)側(cè)額葉及眶額皮層(周曉琳, 高定國等譯,Gazzaniga, Ivry, & Mangun, 2011/2009)。樂觀者正性偏向的神經(jīng)基礎(chǔ)可能涉及前額葉皮質(zhì)(Bangen et al., 2014; De Pascalis et al., 2013)。
樂觀者左側(cè)額上回(left-superior frontal gyrus)的激活會增強(qiáng)。有研究通過腦電圖(EEG)記錄靜息狀態(tài) α波的激活, 發(fā)現(xiàn)樂觀與左側(cè)額上回的激活存在顯著正相關(guān), 與右側(cè)扣帶回后部(the rightposterior cingulate cortex)的激活存在顯著負(fù)相關(guān)。作者認(rèn)為樂觀與這兩個(gè)區(qū)域的相關(guān)不僅反映了他們對趨近與獎勵的敏感性, 還反映了樂觀者對能夠獲得獎賞的期待。而左側(cè)額葉激活的增強(qiáng)可能是導(dǎo)致高樂觀的原因(De Pascalis et al., 2013)。
除了左側(cè)額上回外, 前額葉腹內(nèi)側(cè)皮層(ventral medial prefrontal cortex)、前額葉背內(nèi)側(cè)皮層(dorsal medial prefrontal cortex)以及額下回(inferior frontal gyrus) 的激活水平也與樂觀相關(guān)。這些腦區(qū)激活的降低可能與樂觀者對負(fù)性信息的不敏感有關(guān)。有研究發(fā)現(xiàn), 在加工負(fù)性信息的過程中, 這些腦區(qū)的激活會隨著樂觀分?jǐn)?shù)的提高而下降(Bangen et al., 2014)。作者認(rèn)為這是因?yàn)闃酚^降低了負(fù)性信息的凸顯性。而已有研究發(fā)現(xiàn)上述腦區(qū)激活的增強(qiáng)與抑郁癥患者、焦慮癥患者亦或創(chuàng)傷后應(yīng)激障礙患者對負(fù)性刺激的加工增強(qiáng)有關(guān)(Mcclure et al., 2007; Morey, Petty, Cooper, LaBar,& McCarthy, 2008; Strawn et al., 2012)。例如, 相對于中性刺激, 創(chuàng)傷性綜合癥患者在加工負(fù)性信息過程中額下回的激活水平會增強(qiáng)(Morey et al.,2008)。這從反面證明了上述腦區(qū)激活的下降與個(gè)體對負(fù)性信息加工的減弱相關(guān)。也就是說, 前額葉腹內(nèi)側(cè)皮層、前額葉背內(nèi)側(cè)皮層以及額下回激活的降低與樂觀者對負(fù)性信息的不敏感有關(guān)。
總之, 樂觀者表現(xiàn)出正性信息偏向與前扣帶回喙部、前額葉皮質(zhì)的激活水平變化有關(guān)。這些腦區(qū)激活的變化與情緒信息加工相關(guān), 可能反映了正性信息的凸顯性的提高以及負(fù)性信息的凸顯性的降低, 進(jìn)而導(dǎo)致樂觀者表現(xiàn)出正性偏向。不過, 上述研究主要考察的是樂觀者在注意、情緒及期待方面表現(xiàn)出正性偏向的神經(jīng)生理機(jī)制。前文所提到的一些腦區(qū)如前扣帶回喙部、前額葉皮質(zhì)等均是與注意、認(rèn)知控制、情緒調(diào)節(jié)亦或期待相關(guān)的腦結(jié)構(gòu)(Henseler, Krüger, Dechent, &Gruber, 2011)。而且, 研究中所用的實(shí)驗(yàn)范式也是用來考察注意、情緒調(diào)節(jié)或期待, 比如表情匹配任務(wù)或想象未來積極或消極事件(Hariri, Bookheimer,& Mazziotta, 2000)。目前, 尚未有研究考察樂觀者在其他方面, 如知覺、記憶、解釋風(fēng)格及動機(jī)等等, 表現(xiàn)出正性偏向的神經(jīng)生理機(jī)制。
為什么樂觀者和悲觀者前扣帶回喙部和前額葉皮質(zhì)的激活水平會不同?這可能與遺傳有關(guān)。大量雙生子研究發(fā)現(xiàn)樂觀的遺傳度大約為30%(Caprara et al., 2009; Mosing, Zietsch, Shekar,Wright, & Martin, 2009; Mosing, Pedersen, Martin,& Wright, 2010; Plomin et al., 1992)。如 Mosing等人(2009)以3053對50歲至94歲的雙生子為研究對象發(fā)現(xiàn)樂觀的遺傳度為36%。同年, Caprara等人以428對23歲至24歲的雙生子為研究對象發(fā)現(xiàn)樂觀的遺傳度為 28%。近年, 學(xué)者還開始考察是否存在特定的基因成分直接影響樂觀和悲觀,但是結(jié)果并不一致。如 Saphire-Bernstein, Way,Kim, Sherman和Taylor (2011)的研究發(fā)現(xiàn)催產(chǎn)素受體(OXTR)基因的rs53576 GG型可能是樂觀的生物學(xué)基礎(chǔ), 因?yàn)橄鄬τ趓s53576 AA等位基因攜帶者, GG基因攜帶者的樂觀水平較高。但是, 另一項(xiàng)對美國 1229名女性的研究發(fā)現(xiàn)上述兩組基因攜帶者的樂觀分?jǐn)?shù)并無顯著差異(Cornelis et al.,2012)。 Strohmaier等人則(2013)發(fā)現(xiàn), 男性鈣離子通道基因(CACNA1)中的rs1006737 AA等位基因攜帶者的樂觀水平較低。總之, 樂觀和悲觀的差異部分源于遺傳, 而未來研究還需深入考察是否存在特定的基因(如催產(chǎn)素受體基因的 rs53576 GG型等)對樂觀具有獨(dú)特的作用。
綜上所述, 樂觀給個(gè)體帶來諸多好處是因?yàn)闃酚^者具有正性偏向, 并主要體現(xiàn)在注意、知覺、記憶、解釋風(fēng)格、應(yīng)對策略的選擇、情緒、動機(jī)及期待方面。而這種偏向的神經(jīng)機(jī)制可能與前扣帶回喙部、前額葉皮質(zhì)激活水平密切相關(guān)。盡管有關(guān)樂觀心理機(jī)制的研究已經(jīng)取得上述成果, 但是該領(lǐng)域仍存在不足之處, 未來研究需要注意以下三點(diǎn):
首先, 未來研究應(yīng)該著重考察樂觀核心認(rèn)知結(jié)構(gòu)——期待——本身的認(rèn)知機(jī)制。樂觀從本質(zhì)上來說是一種認(rèn)知結(jié)構(gòu), 其核心是期待。借助期待結(jié)果的好壞, 這種維度也包含著情緒的意味(Carver & Scheier, 2014)。而當(dāng)前樂觀的研究基本上是通過劃分不同樂觀程度的受試者來考察樂觀者和悲觀者在認(rèn)知加工過程中的不同表現(xiàn), 進(jìn)而探討樂觀的心理機(jī)制(Levens & Gotlib, 2012;Gibson & Sanbonmatsu, 2004), 較少對樂觀核心的認(rèn)知結(jié)構(gòu)——期待——本身進(jìn)行深入考察。當(dāng)然,這主要是因?yàn)槲覀冊趯?shí)驗(yàn)室很難對特質(zhì)樂觀進(jìn)行嚴(yán)格的操縱。但是氣質(zhì)性樂觀的概念卻給我們提供了一個(gè)新的研究思路, 即對個(gè)體的期待進(jìn)行操縱。我們可以在實(shí)驗(yàn)室條件下通過操控某些變量使被試產(chǎn)生一種積極或消極的期待, 然后考察不同期待對認(rèn)知過程的影響, 從而揭示樂觀認(rèn)知機(jī)制。Segerstrom (2011)曾提到在樂觀作為一種人格特質(zhì)得到深入研究之前, 心理學(xué)家就曾經(jīng)通過操作期待來使人們變得更樂觀或更悲觀。如Carver,Blaney和Scheier (1979)通過指導(dǎo)語對期待進(jìn)行操作, 發(fā)現(xiàn)誘發(fā)出積極期待的個(gè)體解決難題的時(shí)間顯著長于那些誘發(fā)出消極期待的個(gè)體。未來, 我們可以延長線索提示范式中的線索提示與預(yù)期刺激(正性或負(fù)性)之間的時(shí)間間隔, 并且在其中安排某些認(rèn)知任務(wù), 這樣便可通過不同情緒期待下被試完成認(rèn)知任務(wù)的差異來考察積極期待對認(rèn)知的影響, 進(jìn)而深入探討樂觀的認(rèn)知心理機(jī)制。
其次, 未來研究應(yīng)該合理劃分樂觀和悲觀。已有的樂觀實(shí)驗(yàn)研究大多單純以悲觀為基線考察樂觀的心理機(jī)制, 這可能不夠全面。Scheier和Carver (1985)提出氣質(zhì)性樂觀時(shí)認(rèn)為樂觀和悲觀是一個(gè)維度的兩端, 但是現(xiàn)有研究發(fā)現(xiàn)樂觀和悲觀這兩個(gè)維度之間僅呈低相關(guān)(Herzberg, Glaesmer,& Hoyer, 2006; 溫娟娟, 2012)。而且, 個(gè)體可以同時(shí)具有高樂觀和高悲觀這兩種特質(zhì)傾向(Benyamini,2005; 陶沙, 2006)。這說明, 樂觀的反面可能是缺乏樂觀而并非悲觀(Marshall, Wortman, Kusulas,Hervig, & Vickers, 1992)。同理, 悲觀的反面也不是樂觀, 而是缺乏悲觀(Robinson-Whelen, Kim,MacCallum, & Kiecolt-Glaser, 1997)。缺乏樂觀與存在悲觀可能會造成不同的后果(Norem & Chang,2001)。所以, 這就意味我們不能僅依據(jù)LOT-R問卷總分簡單地將個(gè)體歸為樂觀者或悲觀者。未來,一方面, 我們可以依據(jù)樂觀LOT-R問卷兩個(gè)維度的得分高低劃分不同氣質(zhì)性樂觀類型的個(gè)體來考察樂觀的心理機(jī)制。如有研究據(jù)此分類, 發(fā)現(xiàn)具有高樂觀低悲觀傾向的個(gè)體占總體的 35.9%; 同時(shí)具有高樂觀高悲觀傾向的個(gè)體占 19.8%; 不具有明顯樂觀悲觀傾向的個(gè)體占 21%; 具有低樂觀高悲觀傾向的個(gè)體占 23.4%, 并且上述 4種類型在壓力和抑郁量表上的得分具有顯著差異(陶沙,2006)。這說明, 不同氣質(zhì)性樂觀類型的劃分是有必要的。另一方面, 在進(jìn)行回歸預(yù)測分析時(shí), 我們不僅以樂觀LOT-R的總分為預(yù)測變量, 還可以以各維度分?jǐn)?shù)為預(yù)測變量。如有研究以樂觀維度和悲觀維度的分?jǐn)?shù)為預(yù)測變量發(fā)現(xiàn), 悲觀維度而非樂觀維度的分?jǐn)?shù)能夠顯著預(yù)測工作記憶中情緒信息的連接和打破能力(Levens & Gotlib, 2012)。
再次, 未來研究應(yīng)該采取事件相關(guān)電位(ERP)技術(shù)考察樂觀發(fā)生作用的時(shí)間特征。樂觀發(fā)生的作用可能會隨著時(shí)間而變化。比如樂觀是對未來的積極期待, 但是當(dāng)所期待的事件未能實(shí)現(xiàn)時(shí),樂觀者會感到更多的失望或悲傷嗎?有研究發(fā)現(xiàn)面對落空的期待, 相對于消極期待者, 積極期待者感知到的負(fù)面情緒并沒有顯著增加(Brown &Marshall, 2001)。這有可能是因?yàn)槠诖淇罩?樂觀者能夠迅速地調(diào)整自己的情緒。不過因?yàn)檫@一結(jié)果是通過調(diào)查研究獲得的, 可能由于其所測量情緒的滯后性, 所以研究并未發(fā)現(xiàn)樂觀者與悲觀者所經(jīng)歷情緒的差異。而且到目前為止, 考察樂觀神經(jīng)生理機(jī)制的研究均是采用fMRI或EEG的方法(De Pascalis et al., 2013; Sharot et al., 2007),主要是從大腦空間上揭示了樂觀的神經(jīng)生理基礎(chǔ),卻沒有采用高時(shí)間分辨率的事件相關(guān)電位技術(shù)(ERP)考察樂觀發(fā)生作用的時(shí)間特征。
總之, 未來研究應(yīng)進(jìn)一步考察樂觀的核心即期待的作用機(jī)制。這種期待既有情緒的意味也有動機(jī)的成分, 從而產(chǎn)生對未來及現(xiàn)在正在進(jìn)行任務(wù)的影響。而這些是探究樂觀心理機(jī)制的核心。我們對基于此發(fā)展的樂觀人格特質(zhì)的考察, 也可以不必拘泥于樂觀和悲觀的兩級劃分, 而將對未來的期待作為核心部分。另外, 神經(jīng)機(jī)制方面, 樂觀與涉及情緒與動機(jī)信息加工的腦結(jié)構(gòu)如前扣帶回喙部、前額葉皮質(zhì)的激活水平存在顯著相關(guān)。由此, 未來研究可以將期待?價(jià)值理論、人格理論與神經(jīng)基礎(chǔ)全面整合, 深入且全面考察樂觀的心理機(jī)制。
李恒芬, 曹素霞, 張勇. (2006). 焦慮抑郁中學(xué)生的家庭環(huán)境和認(rèn)知傾向.中國心理衛(wèi)生雜志, 20(9), 582?585.
李燚. (2011).氣質(zhì)性樂觀大學(xué)生的信息加工偏向(碩士學(xué)位論文). 河南大學(xué).
劉孟超, 黃希庭. (2013). 希望: 心理學(xué)的研究述評.心理科學(xué)進(jìn)展, 21(3), 548?560.
祁珍華, 陳瑨, 江虹, 張紅靜. (2012). 大學(xué)生的歸因方式及靈活性與心理健康.中國心理衛(wèi)生雜志, 26(4),310?314.
陶沙. (2006). 樂觀、悲觀傾向與抑郁的關(guān)系及壓力、性別的調(diào)節(jié)作用.心理學(xué)報(bào), 38(6), 886?901.
溫娟娟. (2012). 生活定向測驗(yàn)在大學(xué)生中的信效度.中國心理衛(wèi)生雜志, 26(4), 305?309.
溫娟娟, 鄭雪. (2011). 大學(xué)生氣質(zhì)性樂觀、解釋風(fēng)格與主觀幸福感的關(guān)系研究.心理研究, 4(5), 90?96.
溫娟娟, 鄭雪, 張靈. (2007). 國外樂觀研究述評.心理科學(xué)進(jìn)展, 15(1), 129?133.
楊慧, 吳明證, 劉永芳, Li Q. (2012). 自尊與記憶偏向的關(guān)系.心理科學(xué), 35(4), 962?967.
周曉虹. (1997).現(xiàn)代社會心理學(xué): 多維視野中的社會行為研究. 上海人民出版社.
Abele, A. E., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2007). Individual differences in optimism predict the recall of personally relevant information.Personality and Individual Differences,43(5), 1125?1135.
Alarcon, G. M., Bowling, N. A., & Khazon, S. (2013). Great expectations: A meta-analytic examination of optimism and hope.Personality and Individual Differences, 54(7),821?827.
Ammirati, R. J., Lamis, D. A., Campos, P. E., & Farber, E.W. (2015). Optimism, well-being, and perceived stigma in individuals living with HIV.AIDS Care: Psychological and Socio-medical Aspects of AIDS/HIV, 27(7), 926?933.
Aspinwall, L. G., & Brunhart, S. M. (1996). Distinguishing optimism from denial: Optimistic beliefs predict attention to health threats.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(10), 993?1003.
Aspinwall, L. G., Richter, L., & Hoffman, R. R. (2001).Understanding how optimism works: An examination of optimists' adaptive moderation of belief and behavior. In E.C. Chang (Ed.),Optimism & pessimism: Implications for theory, research, and practice(pp. 217?238). Washington,DC: American Psychological Association.
Bangen, K. J., Bergheim, M., Kaup, A. R., Mirzakhanian, H.,Wierenga, C. E., Jeste, D. V., & Eyler, L. T. (2014).Brains of optimistic older adults respond less to fearful faces.The Journal of Neuropsychiatry & Clinical Neurosciences,26(2), 155?163.
Beard, K. S., Hoy, W. K., & Woolfolk Hoy, A. (2010).Academic optimism of individual teachers: Confirming a new construct.Teaching and Teacher Education, 26(5),1136–1144.
Benyamini, Y. (2005). Can high optimism and high pessimism co-exist? Findings from arthritis patients coping with pain.Personality and Individual Differences, 38(6), 1463?1473.
Benyamini, Y., & Raz, O. (2007). “I can tell you if I'll really lose all that weight”: Dispositional and situated optimism as predictors of weight loss following a group intervention.Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 37(4), 844–861.
Boelen, P. A. (2015). Optimism in prolonged grief and depression following loss: A three-wave longitudinal study.Psychiatry Research, 227(2-3), 313?317.
Brown, J. D., & Marshall, M. A. (2001). Great expectations:Optimism and pessimism in achievement settings. In E. C.Chang (Ed.),Optimism & pessimism: Implications for theory, research, and practice(pp. 239?255). Washington,DC: American Psychological Association.
Bryant, F. B., & Cvengros, J. A. (2004). Distinguishing hope and optimism: Two sides of a coin, or two separate coins?Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 23(2), 273?302.
Büyüka?ik-?olak, C., Gündo?du-Aktürk, E., & Bozo, ?.(2012). Mediating role of coping in the dispositional optimism–posttraumatic growth relation in breast cancer patients.The Journal of Psychology, 146(5), 471?483.
Caprara, G. V., Fagnani, C., Alessandri, G., Steca, P.,Gigantesco, A., Sforza, L. L. C., & Stazi, M. A. (2009).Human optimal functioning: The genetics of positive orientation towards self, life, and the future.Behavior Genetics, 39(3), 277?284.
Carron, A. V., Shapcott, K. M., & Martin, L. J. (2014). The relationship between team explanatory style and team success.International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 12(1), 1?9.
Carver, C. S., Blaney, P. H., & Scheier, M. F. (1979).Reassertion and giving up: The interactive role of self-directed attention and outcome expectancy.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(10), 1859?1870.
Carver, C. S., Pozo, C., Harris, S. D., Noriega, V., Scheier,M. F., Robinson, D. S.,.... Clark, K. C. (1993). How coping mediates the effect of optimism on distress: A study of women with early stage breast cancer.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(2), 375?389.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2001).On the self-regulation of behavior. Cambridge University Press.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2014). Dispositional optimism.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(6), 293?299.
Chang, Y. S., & Chan, H. J. (2015). Optimism and proactive coping in relation to burnout among nurses.Journal of Nursing Management, 23(3), 401?408.
Cisler, J. M., & Koster, E. H. W. (2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review.Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2),203?216.
Cornelis, M.C., Glymour, M. M., Chang, S. C., Tchetgen, E.J. T., Liang, L., Koenen, K. C., … Kubzansky, L. D.(2012). Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) is not associated with optimism in the Nurses’ Health Study.Molecular Psychiatry,17(12), 1157–1159.
Cristea, I. A., Sucala, M., Stefan, S., Igna, R., David, D., &Tatar, A. S. (2011). Positive and negative emotions in cardiac patients: The contributions of trait optimism,expectancies and hopes.Cognitie, Creier, Comportament/Cognition, Brain, Behavior, 15(3), 317?329.
De Pascalis, V., Cozzuto, G., Caprara, G. V., & Alessandri, G.(2013). Relations among EEG-alpha asymmetry, BIS/BAS,and dispositional optimism.Biological Psychology, 94(1),198?209.
Gaudreau, P., Gunnell, K. E., Hoar, S. D., Thompson, A., &Lelièvre, J. (2015). Optimism, pessimism, and coping in a dual-domain model of sport and school satisfaction.Sport,Exercise, and Performance Psychology, 4(2), 140–152.
Gazzaniga, M. S., Ivry, R. B., & Mangun, G. R. (2011).Cognitive neuroscience: The biology of the mind(X. L.Zhou, D. G. Gao, et al., Trans.). Beijing: China Light Industry Press. (Original work published 2009).
[Gazzaniga, M. S., Ivry, R. B., & Mangun, G. R. (2011).認(rèn)知神經(jīng)科學(xué): 關(guān)于心智的神經(jīng)生物學(xué)(周曉琳, 高定國等譯). 北京: 中國輕工業(yè)出版社.]
Geers, A. L., Wellman, J. A., Helfer, S. G., Fowler, S. L., &France, C. R. (2008). Dispositional optimism and thoughts of well-being determine sensitivity to an experimental pain task.Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 36(3), 304?313.
Gibson, B., & Sanbonmatsu, D. M. (2004). Optimism,pessimism, and gambling: The downside of optimism.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(2), 149?160.
Gil, S., & Weinberg, M. (2015). Coping strategies and internal resources of dispositional optimism and mastery as predictors of traumatic exposure and of PTSD symptoms:A prospective study.Psychological Trauma Theory Research Practice & Policy, 7(4), 405?411.
Glaesmer, H., Hoyer, J., Klotsche, J., & Herzberg, P. Y.(2008). Die deutsche version des Life-Orientation-Tests(LOT-R) zum dispositionellen optimismus und pessimismus.Zeitschrift für Gesundheitspsychologie, 16, 26?31.
Goodarzi, A., Shokri, O., & Sharifi, M. (2015). Cognitive appraisals, coping strategies, optimism and subjective well-being.Journal of Psychology, 18(4), 346?364.
Goodin, B. R., Glover, T. L., Sotolongo, A., King, C. D.,Sibille, K. T., Herbert, M. S.,.... Fillingim, R. B. (2013).The association of greater dispositional optimism with less endogenous pain facilitation is indirectly transmitted through lower levels of pain catastrophizing.The Journalof Pain, 14(2), 126?135.
Goodin, B. R., Kronfli, T., King, C. D., Glover, T. L., Sibille,K., & Fillingim, R. B. (2013). Testing the relation between dispositional optimism and conditioned pain modulation:Does ethnicity matter?.Journal of Behavioral Medicine,36(2), 165?174.
Hanssen, M. M., Peters, M. L., Vlaeyen, J. W. S., Meevissen,Y. M. C., & Vancleef, L. M. G. (2013). Optimism lowers pain: Evidence of the causal status and underlying mechanisms.Pain, 154(1), 53?58.
Hariri, A. R., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Mazziotta, J. C. (2000).Modulating emotional responses: Effects of a neocortical network on the limbic system.Neuroreport, 11(1), 43?48.
Helfer, S. G., Elhai, J. D., & Geers, A. L. (2015). Affect and exercise: Positive affective expectations can increase post-exercise mood and exercise intentions.Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 49(2), 269?279.
Henseler, I., Krüger, S., Dechent, P., & Gruber, O. (2011). A gateway system in rostral PFC? Evidence from biasing attention to perceptual information and internal representations.NeuroImage, 56(3), 1666?1676.
Herzberg, P. Y., Glaesmer, H., & Hoyer, J. (2006).Separating optimism and pessimism: A robust psychometric analysis of the revised Life Orientation Test (LOT-R).Psychological Assessment, 18(4), 433?438.
Hingle, M. D., Wertheim, B. C., Tindle, H. A., Tinker, L.,Seguin, R. A., Rosal, M. C., & Thomson, C. A. (2014).Optimism and diet quality in the women's health initiative.Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics, 114(7),1036?1045.
Hirsch, J. K., Nsamenang, S. A., Chang, E. C., & Kaslow, N.J. (2014). Spiritual well-being and depressive symptoms in female African American suicide attempters: Mediating effects of optimism and pessimism.Psychology of Religion & Spirituality, 6(4), 276?283.
Hirsch, J. K., & Rabon, J K. (2015). Optimistic explanatory style and suicide attempt in young adults.International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 13(6), 675?686.
Hood, A., Pulvers, K., Carrillo, J., Merchant, G., & Thomas,M. D. (2012). Positive traits linked to less pain through lower pain catastrophizing.Personality and Individual Differences, 52(3), 401?405.
Hunter, A. M., Korb, A. S., Cook, I. A., & Leuchter, A. F.(2013). Rostral anterior cingulate activity in major depressive disorder: State or trait marker of responsiveness to medication?The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 25(2), 126?133.
Iida, S., Nakao, T., & Ohira, H. (2012). Prior cognitive activity implicitly modulates subsequent emotional responses to subliminally presented emotional stimuli.Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 12(2),337?345.
Ilies, R., & Judge, T. A. (2005). Goal regulation across time:The effects of feedback and affect.Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(3), 453?467.
Isaacowitz, D. M. (2005). The gaze of the optimist.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(3), 407?415.
Jobin, J., Wrosch, C., & Scheier, M. F. (2014). Associations between dispositional optimism and diurnal cortisol in a community sample: When stress is perceived as higher than normal.Health Psychology, 33(4), 382?391.
Johnson, J. M., & Endler, N. S. (2002). Coping with human immunodeficiency virus: Do optimists fare better?Current Psychology, 21(1), 3?16.
Karademas, E. C., Kafetsios, K., & Sideridis, G. D. (2007).Optimism, self-efficacy and information processing of threat- and well-being-related stimuli.Stress and Health,23(5), 285?294.
Kwok, S. Y. C. L., Cheng, L., & Wong, D. F. K. (2015).Family emotional support, positive psychological capital and job satisfaction among Chinese white-collar workers.Journal of Happiness Studies, 16(3), 561?582.
Laranjeira, C. A. (2008). Portuguese translation and validation of the revised life orientation test (LOT-R).Universitas Psychologica,7(2), 469?476.
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984).Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York: Springer.
Lee, H., & Mason, D. (2013). Optimism and coping strategies among Caucasian, Korean, and African American older women.Health Care for Women International,34(12), 1084?1096.
Lench, H. C. (2011). Personality and health outcomes:Making positive expectations a reality.Journal of Happiness Studies,12(3), 493?507.
Levens, S. M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2012). The effects of optimism and pessimism on updating emotional information in working memory. Cognition & Emotion, 26(2), 341?350.
Luo, J. (2008).Optimism, cognition, and information processing in medical decision making(Unpublished doctorial dissertation). Brandeis University, United States of America.
Maleva, V., Westcott, K., McKellop, M., McLaughlin, R., &Widman, D. (2014). Optimism and college grades:Predicting GPA from explanatory style.Psi Chi Journal of Psychological Research, 19(3), 129?135.
Marshall, G. N., Wortman, C. B., Kusulas, J. W., Hervig, L.K., & Vickers, R. R., Jr. (1992). Distinguishing optimism from pessimism: Relations to fundamental dimensions of mood and personality.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(6), 1067?1074.
Mcclure, E. B., Monk, C. S., Nelson, E. E., Parrish, J. M.,Adler, A., Blair, R. J. R., … Pine, D. S. (2007). Abnormal attention modulation of fear circuit function in pediatric generalized anxiety disorder.Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(1), 97?106.
Mchale, N., Clark, D. A., & Tramonte, L. (2015). Does optimism moderate mood repair? A daily diary study.Motivation & Emotion,39(3), 409?419.
McNulty, J. K., & Karney, B. R. (2004). Positive expectations in the early years of marriage: Should couples expect the best or brace for the worst?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(5), 729?743.
Monzani, D., Steca, P., Greco, A., D’Addario, M., Pancani,L., & Cappelletti, E. (2015). Effective pursuit of personal goals: The fostering effect of dispositional optimism on goal commitment and goal progress.Personality &Individual Differences, 82, 203–214.
Morey, R. A., Petty, C. M., Cooper, D. A., LaBar, K. S., &McCarthy, G. (2008). Neural systems for executive and emotional processing are modulated by symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder in Iraq War veterans.Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 162(1), 59–72.
Mosing, M. A., Pedersen, N. L., Martin, N. G., & Wright, M.J. (2010). Sex differences in the genetic architecture of optimism and health and their interrelation: A study of Australian and Swedish twins.Twin Research and Human Genetics, 13(4), 322?329.
Mosing, M. A., Zietsch, B. P., Shekar, S. N., Wright, M. J., &Martin, N. G. (2009). Genetic and environmental influences on optimism and its relationship to mental and self-rated health: A study of aging twins.Behavior Genetics, 39(6), 597?604.
Nieuwenhuis, S., Slagter, H. A., Von Geusau, N. J. A.,Heslenfeld, D. J., & Holroyd, C. B. (2005). Knowing good from bad: Differential activation of human cortical areas by positive and negative outcomes.European Journal of Neuroscience,21(11), 3161?3168.
Norem, J. K., & Chang, E. C. (2001). A very full glass:Adding complexity to our thinking about the implications and applications of optimism and pessimism research. In E.C. Chang (Ed.),Optimism & pessimism: Implications for theory, research, and practice(pp. 347?367). Washington,DC, US: American Psychological Association.
Penedo, F. J., Dahn, J. R., Gonzalez, J. S., Molton, I., Carver,C. S., Antoni, M. H., … Schneiderman, N. (2003).Perceived stress management skill mediates the relationship between optimism and positive mood following radical prostatectomy.Health Psychology,22(2), 220?222.
Perera, H. N., & McIlveen, P. (2014). The role of optimism and engagement coping in college adaptation: A career construction model.Journal of Vocational Behavior, 84(3),395?404.
Peters, M. L., Flink, I. K., Boersma, K., & Linton, S. J.(2010). Manipulating optimism: Can imagining a best possible self be used to increase positive future expectancies?The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5(3), 204?211.
Peterson, C. (2000). The future of optimism.American Psychologist, 55(1), 44?55
Peterson, C., & Barrett, L. C. (1987). Explanatory style and academic performance among university freshman.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(3),603?607.
Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk factor for depression: Theory and evidence.Psychological Review, 91(3), 347?374.
Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004).Character strengths and virtues: A handbook and classification.Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Plomin, R., Scheier, M. F., Bergeman, C. S., Pedersen, N. L.,Nesselroade, J. R., & McClearn, G. E. (1992). Optimism,pessimism and mental health: A twin/adoption analysis.Personality and Individual Differences, 13(8), 921–930.
Raila, H., Scholl, B. J., & Gruber, J. (2015). Seeing the world through rose-colored glasses: People who are happy and satisfied with life preferentially attend to positive stimuli.Emotion, 15(4), 449?462.
Rajandram, R. K., Ho, S. M. Y., Samman, N., Chan, N.,McGrath, C., & Zwahlen, R. A. (2011). Interaction of hope and optimism with anxiety and depression in a specific group of cancer survivors: A preliminary study.BMC Research Notes, 4(1), 519.
Rand, K. L. (2009). Hope and optimism: Latent structures and influences on grade expectancy and academic performance.Journal of Personality, 77(1), 231?260.
Robinson-Whelen, S., Kim, C., MacCallum, R. C., &Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1997). Distinguishing optimism from pessimism in older adults: Is it more important to be optimistic or not to be pessimistic?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(6), 1345?1353.
Rusting, C. L., & Larsen, R. J. (1998). Personality and cognitive processing of affective information.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(2), 200?213.
Rygula, R., Golebiowska, J., Kregiel, J., Kubik, J., & Popik,P. (2015). Effects of optimism on motivation in rats.Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 32.
Sanjuan, P., & Magallares, A. (2015). Coping strategies as mediator variables between explanatory styles and depressive symptoms.Anales de Psicología, 31(2), 447?451.
Saphire-Bernstein, S., Way, B. M., Kim, H. S., Sherman, D.K., & Taylor, S. E. (2011). Oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR)is related to psychological resources.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciencesof the United States of America, 108(37), 15118?15122.
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping,and health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies.Health Psychology, 4(3), 219?247.
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1992). Effects of optimism on psychological and physical well-being: Theoretical overview and empirical update.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16(2), 201?228.
Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994).Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): A reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 1063?1078.
Schulman, P., Castellon, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1989).Assessing explanatory style: The content analysis of verbatim explanations and the attributional style questionnaire.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 27(5), 505?509.
Segerstrom, S. C. (2001). Optimism and attentional bias for negative and positive stimuli.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(10), 1334?1343.
Segerstrom, S. C. (2011).Breaking Murphy's law: How optimists get what they want from life-and pessimists can too. New York: Guilford Press.
Segerstrom, S. C., Taylor S. E., Kemeny M. E., & Fahey J. L.(1998). Optimism is associated with mood, coping, and immune change in response to stress.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,74(6), 1646?1655.
Seligman, M. E. P. (2010).Learned optimism(L. Hong,Trans.). Shenyang: Volumes Publishing Company.(Original work published 1991).
[馬丁·塞利格曼. (2010).活出最樂觀的自己(洪蘭譯). 沈陽: 萬卷出版公司.]
Seligman, M. E., & Schulman, P. (1986). Explanatory style as a predictor of productivity and quitting among life insurance sales agents.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(4), 832?838.
Sezgin, F., & Erdogan, O. (2015). Academic optimism, hope and zest for work as predictors of teacher self-efficacy and perceived success.Educational Sciences: Theory &Practice, 15(1), 7?19.
Shand, L. K., Cowlishaw, S., Brooker, J. E., Burney, S., &Ricciardelli, L. A. (2015). Correlates of post-traumatic stress symptoms and growth in cancer patients: A systematic review and meta-analysis.Psycho-Oncology, 24(6), 624?634.
Sharot, T., Riccardi, A. M., Raio, C. M., & Phelps, E. A.(2007). Neural mechanisms mediating optimism bias.Nature,450(7166), 102?105.
Snyder, C. R. (2002). Hope theory: Rainbows in the mind.Psychological Inquiry, 13(4), 249?275.
Snyder, C. R., Feldman, D. B., Taylor, J. D., Schroeder, L. L.,& Adams, V. H. (2000). The roles of hopeful thinking in preventing problems and enhancing strengths.Applied and Preventive Psychology, 9(4), 249?269.
Snyder, C. R., Harris, C., Anderson, J. R., Holleran, S. A.,Irving, L. M., Sigmon, S. T.,.... Harney, P. (1991). The will and the ways: Development and validation of an individual-differences measure of hope.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(4), 570?585.
Solberg Nes, L., Evans, D. R., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2009).Optimism and college retention: Mediation by motivation,performance, and adjustment.Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 39(8), 1887?1912.
Srivastava, S., McGonigal, K. M., Richards, J. M., Butler, E.A., & Gross, J. J. (2006). Optimism in close relationships:How seeing things in a positive light makes them so.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(1),143?153.
Steca, P., Monzani, D., Greco, A., Chiesi, F., & Primi, C.(2015). Item response theory analysis of the life orientation test-revised: Age and gender differential item functioning analyses.Assessment, 22(3), 341?350.
Strawn, J. R., Bitter, S. M., Weber, W. A., Chu, W. J.,Whitsel, R. M., Adler, C., … DelBello, M. P. (2012).Neurocircuitry of generalized anxiety disorder in adolescents: A pilot functional neuroimaging and functional connectivity study.Depression & Anxiety,29(11), 939?947.
Strohmaier, J., Amelang, M., Hothorn, L. A., Witt, S. H.,Nieratschker, V., Gerhard, D.,.... Schulze, T. G. (2013).The psychiatric vulnerability gene CACNA1C and its sex-specific relationship with personality traits, resilience factors and depressive symptoms in the general population.Molecular Psychiatry, 18(5), 607?613.
Strümpfer, D. J. W. (2006). Positive emotions, positive emotionality and their contribution to fortigenic living: A review.South African Journal of Psychology,36(1),144?167.
Summerfield, C., & Egner, T. (2009). Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(9), 403?409.
Taylor, Z. E., Larsen-Rife, D., Conger, R. D., Widaman, K.F., & Cutrona, C. E. (2010). Life stress, maternal optimism, and adolescent competence in single mother,African American families.Journal of Family Psychology,24(4), 468?477.
Taylor, Z. E., Widaman, K. F., Robins, R. W., Jochem, R.,Early, D. R., & Conger, R. D. (2012). Dispositional optimism: A psychological resource for Mexican-origin mothers experiencing economic stress.Journal of Family Psychology,26(1), 133?139.
Thompson, A., & Gaudreau, P. (2008). From optimism and pessimism to coping: The mediating role of academic motivation.International Journal of Stress Management,15(3), 269?288.
Vogt, B. A., & Pandya, D. N. (1987). Cingulate cortex of the rhesus monkey: II. Cortical afferents.Journal of Comparative Neurology,262(2), 271–289.
Vollmann, M., Antoniw, K., Hartung, F. M., & Renner, B.(2011). Social support as mediator of the stress buffering effect of optimism: The importance of differentiating the recipients' and providers' perspective.European Journal of Personality, 25(2), 146?154.
Warner, L. M., Schwarzer, R., Schüz, B., Wurm, S., &Tesch-R?mer, C. (2012). Health-specific optimism mediates between objective and perceived physical functioning in older adults.Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 35(4), 400?406.
Wimberly, S. R., Carver, C. S., & Antoni, M. H. (2008).Effects of optimism, interpersonal relationships, and distress on psychosexual well-being among women with early stage breast cancer.Psychology and Health, 23(1),57?72.
Wright, M. A., Wren, A. A., Somers, T. J., Goetz, M. C.,Fras, A. M., Huh, B. K.,.... Keefe, F. J. (2011). Pain acceptance, hope, and optimism: Relationships to pain and adjustment in patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain.The Journal of Pain, 12(11), 1155?1162.
Wrosch, C., & Scheier, M. F. (2003). Personality and quality of life: The importance of optimism and goal adjustment.Quality of Life Research, 12(S1), 59?72.
Zaslavsky, O., Palgi, Y., Rillamas-Sun, E., LaCroix, A. Z.,Schnall, E., Woods, N. F.,.... Shrira, A. (2015).Dispositional optimism and terminal decline in global quality of life.Developmental Psychology, 51(6),856?863.
Zhang, J. X., Miao, D. M., Sun, Y. F., Xiao, R. R., Ren, I.,Xiao, W., & Peng, J. X. (2014). The impacts of attributional styles and dispositional optimism on subject well-being: A structural equation modelling analysis.Social Indicators Research, 119(2), 757?769.