• 
    

    
    

      99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看

      ?

      Analysis of Pragmatic Conditions of Null Subject in Mandarin

      2019-09-10 17:10:09LIANGXinyuan
      校園英語·月末 2019年11期
      關(guān)鍵詞:語類空位賓語

      Ⅰ.Background of null subject acquisition research

      Han Jingquan (1997) takes the phenomenon of null object as discourse deletion which should be included in the discourse grammar, because the analysis of the null element beyond the boundary of sentence. So the pragmatic analysis of null subject is necessary, especially for Chinese. Li (1997) regards Chinese as a language with no grammatical requirement for the appearance of referent.

      In this study from a longitudinal database of a Mandarin speaking child from 0;8-4;5 and five records were chosen.

      1. Input. Parameter in UG indicates that children have innate language knowledge which has different parameter settings cross languages. Chinese is [+pro drop] [+zero topic] language (Hickman and Hendricks, 1999) allowing null subject in adult speech. When talking to children, caretaker do not always produce full form of the subject. Sometimes adults omit the subject in discourse. But this does not provide an explanation to the acquisition of null subject. But those examples account for small portion of the utterance, with 9 in 0;08.10 and 5 in 1;03.10, and become even smaller with age increasing.

      2. Focus and NP types.Children produce null subject when the focus of questions does not lie in the subject. When the focus is on the VP, an answer of VP will be given without a subject even though the subject is nominal and has been clearly elicited. The focus can be in any positions of the sentence. And this strategy overwhelms all the other rules in the early age.

      The NP types that children use to indicate the information status agree with the property of separate types of NP. In the data, the child will use ‘ta (it)’ representing for the doll that was mentioned by the mother with a nominal ‘wawa (the doll)’. According to Hickman (1998), the predictability of non-determined nominal is the weakest. After introduced to the discourse, it becomes predictable in the context and mutual knowledge between the mother and the child. And The most possible subject to omit is pronominal and the least one is the nominal.

      The final position is a place for a natural focus in Chinese, which has a function of emphasizing and prominence (Liu Danqing and Xu Liejiong, 1988) and the child may put the pronoun at the final place to emphasis.

      3. Topic continuity. When the topic remains unchanged, the child tend to use more predictable pronouns for the referent. According to Ping Chen (1984), the sentences in a topic chain share the same topic. Chinese is a ‘topic-oriented’ language (Han Jingquan, 1997). Children use null anaphor refer to the pronominals, and all the utterances share a common topic. Hickman (1998) found discourse, cognitive and linguistic factors to relate to newness marking. when the topic remains unchanged, children tend to use null subject for reference and otherwise use an overt subject. But Children do not always use null subject when there is a topic maintenance. It is maybe related to the increase of age. The cognitive requirement increase the complexity of both production and comprehension.

      Ping Chen (1984) gave a pattern of the zero anaphora in subordinate clauses. In the analysis of the data, the child did not produce subordinate clause until 3, and children will not omit the subject in subordinate clauses with an overt main clause subject.

      Ⅱ.Conclusion

      The exploration of the developmental data suggests some regulation of null subject acquisition of young children from 8 month to 4-year-old. There are some conclusions to make. Early age children have null subject and the production percentage decline with ages. And the null subjects are influenced by the input, NP types, continuity of topics, information status and inter-clause structure.

      References:

      [1]Chen, Ping.(1984) A Discourse Analysis of Third Person Zero Anaphora in Chinese. Bloomington: Indiana University Linguistics Club.

      [2]Hickman.(1998) On Null Subjects and other Pronouns: Syntactic and Pragmatic Approaches.

      [3]Hickmann, Maya, and James Liang. (1990). “Clause-structure Variation in Chinese Narrative Discourse: A Developmental Analysis.” Linguistics 28, no. 6 : p1167-200.

      [4]Hickmann, Maya, and Henritte Hendriks. (1999). “Cohesion and Anaphora in Childrens Narratives: A Comparison of English, French, German, and Mandarin Chinese.” Journal of Child Language 26, no. 2 : p419-52.

      [5]韓景泉.空語類理論與漢語空位賓語[J].國(guó)外語言學(xué),1997(4):1-14.

      【作者簡(jiǎn)介】LIANG Xinyuan,Chinese University of Hong Kong。

      猜你喜歡
      語類空位賓語
      話語雜糅中的生死觀建構(gòu)——“生前契約”語類分析
      連詞that引導(dǎo)的賓語從句
      賓語從句及練習(xí)
      語類教學(xué)法:理論基礎(chǔ)與改進(jìn)策略
      Zn空位缺陷長(zhǎng)余輝發(fā)光材料Zn1-δAl2O4-δ的研究
      Martin語類理論視角下英語體育新聞的語類分析——以里約奧運(yùn)會(huì)賽事報(bào)道為例
      中考試題中的賓語從句
      語類翻譯探究
      空位
      讀者欣賞(2014年6期)2014-07-03 03:00:48
      說者無心,聽者有意——片談?wù)Z言交際中的空位對(duì)舉
      屏山县| 吉水县| 东乡族自治县| 台东市| 四平市| 武城县| 临漳县| 芦山县| 德化县| 中宁县| 涞水县| 临澧县| 新宾| 无锡市| 墨江| 中方县| 韩城市| 兰西县| 泸州市| 道孚县| 子洲县| 宜都市| 凉城县| 会同县| 博野县| 安达市| 嘉定区| 阿瓦提县| 长宁区| 昔阳县| 尼勒克县| 石楼县| 曲阜市| 获嘉县| 平度市| 化德县| 麻栗坡县| 玉屏| 阜新| 石城县| 舒城县|